Grunting in tennis is the very loud noise, sometimes described as “shrieking” or “screaming”, made by some players while hitting their shots.
It is prominent in both men’s and women’s tennis.
Exceeding noise levels of 100 decibels, the grunting sounds produced by some tennis players when hitting the ball are on a par with motorbikes or chainsaws.
While fans react to these impressive exhalations with either annoyance or amusement, the habit has also been a source of intense debate among professionals.
For instance, Serena Williams has said that she is not bothered by opponents grunting in the heat of the competition.

In contrast, former world number one Martina Navratilova has complained that grunting masks the sound of the racket striking the ball, making it – unfairly – harder to predict the ball’s trajectory.

The question of whether this common complaint is justified has now been examined in a new study by a team of sport psychologists from Friedrich Schiller University, Jena, led by Dr. Florian Müller and Prof. Rouwen Cañal-Bruland.
Does grunting negatively impact an opponent?
There has been some preliminary experimental work that showed grunting may mask important auditory information used by an opponent.
Participants in a study were asked to watch video clips of a professional tennis player striking the ball with or without an accompanying auditory stimulus (a grunt).
Their task was to determine as quickly and accurately as possible whether the ball was being hit to their left or right side.
Results revealed the grunt did impair the speed and accuracy of their directional decision-making.
Taking the results from the lab onto the court, it has been suggested that the 30-millisecond delay in responding when an additional auditory stimulus is present would mean a typical rally shot would be picked up two feet later, relative to when no grunt is present.
This means more time pressure on the opponent and less preparation time, which is certainly not advantageous to their performance.
How a grunt impairs performance is less clear.
As anecdotally suggested by many professional players, a well-timed grunt can mask important auditory information used by a player as the racket strikes the ball.
Another suggestion is that a grunt could draw a player’s attention away from the sound of racket-ball contact to the actual grunt, which in turn may impair their timing.
Finally, a grunt may draw visual attention away from the processing of the visual information conveyed at racket-ball contact. There is currently no clear evidence to support any of these suggestions.
Does grunting enhance hitting performance?
When the impact of a grunt is investigated, there is evidence that hitting performance is enhanced.
Skilled university tennis players were found to hit with a 3.8% increase in groundstroke hitting velocity when they grunted.
For a serve, a 4.9% enhancement in velocity was found among players who grunted. This translated to “grunted serves” being hit 7kph faster than those that were not.
While the increase in hitting velocity came at no additional physiological cost, in relation to perception of effort and energy consumption, there was an increase in force production as measured by muscular activity. Overall this suggests that grunting is performance-enhancing, and is a sustainable strategy over the course of a match.
Experiment with manipulated grunting noises
For this study, the research team conducted a series of experiments in which experienced players were shown video clips of rallies from a professional tennis match.
After observing players hitting the ball, they had to work out the ball’s trajectory and indicate where it would land.
Largely unnoticed by participants, though, the intensity of the grunting noises was manipulated.
Grunting biases anticipation of ball flight
Results indicate that grunting does have an effect – but not the one claimed by Navratilova.
There was no evidence that grunting caused a distraction effect.
In spite of the supposed irritation, participants’ level of error in predicting where the ball would land was the same – regardless of the intensity of the grunts.
Instead, it was shown that the louder the grunting, the further the participants assumed the ball would fly.
This reaction was observed even when the noises could only be heard after the racket had made contact with the ball, as is usual in many professional matches.
“We assume that players account for the physiological benefits provided by grunting,” explains Müller.
Other researchers have demonstrated that forcefully exhaling air activates the abdominal muscles, providing additional strength that enables players to hit harder, making the ball fly faster.
“This possibly explains why an effect can be observed as a result of the grunting, but the ability to anticipate the ball’s trajectory remains unaffected.”
Perception in sport as the interplay of multiple sensory impressions
According to Müller and his colleagues, the results of the study suggest that Navratilova’s claim needs to be reconsidered.
For the sport psychologists, it is also evidence that sensory impressions other than sight are of importance in sport as well, and that scientists should look at these more closely in future.
For this reason, too, they want to stay ‘on the ball’ and investigate the phenomenon further.
To get closer to real-world conditions, in the next step participants will have to catch a tennis ball on the touchscreen in real time.
Ultimately, the experiment could even be conducted during a real match on a tennis court – as long as no one in the neighbourhood is disturbed by excessively loud grunting.
More information: Florian Müller et al, The sound of speed: How grunting affects opponents’ anticipation in tennis, PLOS ONE (2019). DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0214819
Journal information: PLoS ONE
Provided by Friedrich Schiller University of Jena