Violent video games increases kids attitudes to use unlocked guns

0
1442

Shoot ‘em up video games might be making your children far too comfortable with how they approach and handle real-life firearms, a new study argues.

Kids who played a version of Minecraft featuring guns were much more likely to handle a happened-upon but unloaded firearm than kids whose Minecraft game featured swords or no weapons at all, researchers found.

Worse, those kids were also more likely to point the gun at themselves or a playmate and pull the trigger, said lead researcher Brad Bushman, chair of mass communication at Ohio State University.

“This is not a game that is bloody or gory,” Bushman said of Minecraft, “so you have to wonder how more realistic shooting games would affect children’s perceptions regarding the safety of firearms.”

For the study, 250 kids aged 8 to 12 spent 20 minutes playing specially designed versions of Minecraft, a game known for its distinctively blocky, pixelated graphics.

Kids were paired with another playmate and randomly assigned to play one of three versions of Minecraft—one in which guns were used to kill monsters, another where swords were used, and a third that was utterly nonviolent and featured neither weapons nor monsters.

After 20 minutes of play, the children were sent to another room stocked with toys. The room also included a cabinet containing two disabled 9 mm handguns.

There were 220 children in the study who came across the handguns while playing, and what they did after finding the firearms is telling.

About 62% of kids who played Minecraft featuring guns touched the handgun, compared with 57% of the kids who had swords in their video game and 44% of those who played a nonviolent video game, researchers report.

What’s more, twice as many kids who played with crude video game guns wound up aiming the real-life firearm and pulling the trigger at either themselves or their playmate.

About 3% of kids exposed to gun violence in Minecraft pulled the trigger of the real handgun, compared with 1.4% of kids who had video game swords and only 0.14% who played the nonviolent game.

Roughly 1 in every 5 children notified an adult about the firearm, the study discovered. About 6% told an adult without touching the gun, as kids are told to do, and another 16% told an adult but also touched the gun.

The results show how important it is for parents to monitor their children’s video game playing, Bushman said.

“You don’t let your kids eat junk food. The same should be true for media,” Bushman said. “You shouldn’t let them consume junky media.”

The new results jibe with a 2017 study of his, which found that kids who watched movies featuring gun violence were more likely to handle and use guns, Bushman added.

Nearly 1,300 children younger than 18 die every year from shootings, and about 5,800 are treated for gunshot wounds, according to a 2017 study in Pediatrics.

Although it’s important to monitor kids’ media diet, it’s even more crucial to safely store firearms out of the hands of children, said Cassandra Crifasi, an assistant professor with the Johns Hopkins Center for Gun Policy and Research.

Even before video games existed, kids played with toy firearms—water guns or cap guns or finger guns or guns imagined from bits of fallen wood, said Crifasi, who wasn’t involved with the study.

“Kids are innately curious and they’re going to play with these things, so I think it’s important we separate kids from firearms when they aren’t supervised,” Crifasi said. “We know from some national survey data we’ve done that only 55% of homes with children store their guns safely.”

The best way to keep children safe is to remove all guns from the home, according to Nationwide Children’s Hospital in Columbus, Ohio.

Guns kept in the house should always be unloaded and locked, stored out of reach and sight of children. Ammunition should always be stored separately.

The new study was published May 31 in the journal JAMA Network Open.


In a recent study, a team of German researchers led by Simone Kühn took a different approach and focused on the long-term impact of video games on aggression.

Using a longitudinal intervention study design, the researchers tested their hypothesis that long-term exposure to violent video games does not increase aggression or decrease pro-social behaviors.

In their study, Kühn et al. used a sample of 90 college students and adults who had little or no video game exposure during the previous six months.

The participants were then randomly assigned to three groups:

1) A violent video game group in which participants were given a Playstation 3 console to play Grand Theft Auto V;

2) An active control group in which participants were given a Playstation 3 console to play The Sims 3, a video game deemed not to be violent; and a

3) passive control group in which participants were not given a console and were not told that they were an experimental control.

Each participant in the Violent Video Game and Passive Control Groups was required to play their assigned video game for at least thirty minutes each day over a two-month period.

To analyze measures of aggression and pro-social behaviors, the researchers utilized questionnaires and behavioral tasks that had been used in previous research on the effects of video games.

These tests were administered to the participants three times—before the experiment, directly after the two-month required-gaming period, and two months after the experiment.

This study design worked to set a baseline in advance of the experiment and provided information on longer-term effects of video games via data collection two months after video gaming.

Data analysis entailed the collection of 52 different outcome variables across four tests and was tested at a 5-percent significance level.

Accordingly, the researchers expected that as many as 10 analyses would be significant by chance.

Further, as it is known that aggression and empathy are different between males and females and that the different sample groups had varying average age, the data results were controlled for sex and age.

The researchers found no negative effect of violent video gaming.

Across the 208 different tests, the measures of difference in aggression, empathy, and other behaviors across sample groups and time only resulted in three significant results.

Since 10 significant results were expected because of chance, the researchers substantiated their hypothesis – violent video games do not impact the attitudes of players.

This result provides significant social science evidence to counter narratives blaming violent actions on video games. 

While violent video games may have some impact in the immediate short-term, video gaming does not seem to impact the long-term aggressive behaviors and attitudes of adult players. 

However, as many concerns about violence and video games address the well-being of children and teens, a further study targeting younger people may be needed to confirm these results.

Likewise, future studies should look to strengthen external validity of the results through analyses that look at differences across countries, length of study and the number of violent video games being played.

In addition, as violent video game cultures permeate online communities, further research on the effects of video games should address how violent video gaming may impact the online behaviors of gamers.

Article source: Kühn, Simone, Dimitrij Tycho Kugler, Katharina Schmalen, Markus Weichenberger, Charlotte Witt, and Jürgen Gallinat. “Does Playing Violent Video Games Cause Aggression? A Longitudinal Intervention Study.” Molecular Psychiatry (2018).


More information: Justin H. Chang et al. Effect of Exposure to Gun Violence in Video Games on Children’s Dangerous Behavior With Real Guns, JAMA Network Open (2019). DOI: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.4319

Journal information: JAMA Network Open , Pediatrics

LEAVE A REPLY

Please enter your comment!
Please enter your name here

Questo sito usa Akismet per ridurre lo spam. Scopri come i tuoi dati vengono elaborati.