Mothers’ behavior influences oxytocin system in their babies

0
1063

Oxytocin is an extremely important hormone, involved in social interaction and bonding in mammals, including humans.

It helps us relate to others. It strengthens trust, closeness in relationships, and can be triggered by eye contact, empathy, or pleasant touch.

It’s well known that a new mother’s oxytocin levels can influence her behavior and as a result, the bond she makes with her baby.

A new epigenetic study by Kathleen Krol and Jessica Connelly from the University of Virginia and Tobias Grossmann from the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences now suggests that mothers’ behavior can also have a substantial impact on their children’s developing oxytocin systems.

Childhood marks a dynamic and malleable phase of postnatal development.

Many bodily systems are coming online, maturing, or getting tweaked, often setting our psychological and behavioral trajectories well into adulthood.

Nature plays an obvious role, shaping us through our genes.

But we are also heavily influenced by our interactions, with other people, and with our environment.

“It is well known that oxytocin is actively involved in early social, perceptual, and cognitive processes, and, that it influences complex social behaviors,” says Tobias Grossmann.

“However, in this study, we ask whether the mother’s behavior might also have a decisive influence on the development of the baby’s oxytocin system itself.

Advances in molecular biology, epigenetics, in particular, have recently made it possible to investigate the interaction of nature and nurture, in this case, infant care, in fine detail. That is exactly what we’ve done here”.

The scientists observed a free play interaction between mothers and their five-month-old children.

“We collected saliva samples from both the mother and the infant during the visit and then a year later when the child was 18 months old.

We were interested in exploring whether the involvement of the mother, in the original play session, would have an influence on the oxytocin receptor gene of the child, a year later.

The oxytocin receptor is essential for the hormone oxytocin to exert its effects and the gene can determine how many are produced,” explains Kathleen Krol, a Hartwell postdoctoral fellow in Connelly’s Lab at the University of Virginia who conducted the study together with Tobias Grossmann at MPI CBS in Leipzig.

In the study, the scientists observed a free play interaction between mothers and their five-month-old children.

“We found that epigenetic changes had occurred in the infant’s DNA and that this change was predicted by the quality of the mother’s involvement in the play session.

If mothers were particularly involved in the game with their children, there was a greater reduction in DNA methylation of the oxytocin receptor gene one year later.

Decreased DNA methylation in this region has previously been associated with increased expression of the oxytocin receptor gene.

Thus, greater maternal involvement seems to have the potential to upregulate the oxytocin system in human offspring,” explains the scientist.

“Importantly, we also found that the DNA methylation levels reflected infant temperament, which was reported to us by the parents.

The children with higher methylation levels at 18-months and presumably lower levels of oxytocin receptor were also more temperamental and less well balanced.”

The results of this study provide a striking example of how we are not simply bound by our genes but are rather the products of a delicate interplay between our blueprints and experiences.

Early social interaction with our caregivers, certainly not excluding fathers, can influence our biological and psychological development through epigenetic changes to the oxytocin system.

These and related findings highlight the importance of parenting in promoting cross-generational health.


Sensitive and responsive maternal caregiving behavior is important for promoting secure attachment[1] and healthy emotional, cognitive and social development of a child.[2] On the contrary, poor maternal caregiving is associated with poorer outcomes in infants,[3] such as emotional and behavioral problems during childhood[4] and mental health disorders later in life.[5]

Evidence from both animal and human studies have shown that the quality of upbringing received is reflected in the care provided to offspring;[6,7] therefore, the quality of parenting mothers offer to their children may impact the quality of parenting these children later provide to their offspring.

Such transfer of upbringing behavior from one generation to another is through the non-traditional way of inheritance known as “epigenetic.”

Consistent with the findings in animals, studies using imaging technique in humans have shown that experiencing adverse parenting during the early years of life can change the brain morphology.[8,9]

In addition, functional imaging studies have provided concrete evidence that the responses of maternal brain to their infant’s signal (auditory or visual stimuli) may also reflect the quality of perceived parenting.[7,10]

Hormones, such as vasopressin, testosterone, cortisol, oxytocin (OT) and prolactin have been found to play significant roles in shaping maternal caregiving behavior.[11]

The influence of these hormones may be affected by the quality of upbringing.[12] Here, the authors focus on OT as a key factor implicated in social behavior and parenting[13] as well as in brain adaptation to quality of perceived parenting.[14]

This review aims to contribute to the current literature by highlighting neurobiological changes in mothers in relation to quality of upbringing they received.

Specifically, the review focusses on the structural and functional changes in the brain or the brain reactivity in response to infant signals and the pattern for OT reactivity in response to interactive behavior with own infant.

By highlighting neurobiological changes related to early experience, the authors hope to pave the way for studies to identify biomarkers in mothers who experienced adverse experience of upbringing so that they could be targeted by interventions.

Such intervention would aim to alleviate the impact of early experience on maternal behavior toward own child, thereby improving a mother’s parenting behavior and a child’s outcome.

IMPACT OF OWN PERCEIVED PARENTING ON MATERNAL CAREGIVING BEHAVIOR

Cross-fostering animal studies have demonstrated the role of early environment in shaping maternal caregiving behavior toward own offspring.[15,16,17] In rodent, for example, female pups who were born to mothers with “low” quality of maternal caregiving (i.e., licking and grooming [LG]) but reared by “high” LG foster mothers were found to show high levels of parental care when they handled their own pups.[18]

Similarly, when offspring of high LG mothers were reared by low LG foster mothers, they showed low quality of caregiving when handling their pups.[15,18,19]

In humans, earlier studies suggest a positive relationship between perceived parenting and maternal sensitivity (i.e., accurate and prompt responses to infant signals). In a study of 60 mothers, using the adult attachment interview, it was found that mothers with secure attachment to parents were more sensitive to their infant than nonsecure mothers.[20,21] Similar findings were also found when parenting bonding instrument (PBI), a self-reporting measure, was used.[22]

Recently, in a sample of 80 healthy British mothers, maternal perceived parental care (assessed using PBI) was found to be positively correlated with rating of maternal sensitivity when mothers interacted with their 4–6-month-old infants.[12] Similar findings were also reported in another study where 192 British couples were followed up for 2 years after childbirth.

The couples’ interactive behavior with their child and perception of own perceived parenting were assessed and it was found that a “controlling” mother at family of origin was positively associated with decreased “engagement” with own infant.[23]

EPIGENETIC TRANSMISSION OF MATERNAL CAREGIVING BEHAVIOR

Cross-generational transmission of maternal behavior is suggested to occur in an epigenetic manner, and this can be passed down for generations.[24] Mechanism involved in epigenetic changes vary, but generally involve chemical changes (e.g., DNA methylation) in genes that either silence or enhance it.[16,25]

Epigenetic transmission of maternal behavior is due to alterations in the hypothalamic estrogen receptors through methylation.[26] This, in turn, leads to neurobiological changes with respect to social attachments and stress reactivity.[15,27] The effects of these epigenetic changes can be modified by life experiences (e.g., mother–infant interaction).[28]

The above-discussed findings are in line with the “internal working model” of the attachment theory,[29] according to which positive care experiences in the early years of life provide individuals with the emotional and cognitive resources that enhance their social learning to subsequently provide high-quality caregiving.[30]

Transgenerational transmission of parental behavior is related with interaction between environment (i.e., quality of caregiving received) and genotypes, resulting in heritable changes in gene expression.

NEUROBIOLOGICAL IMPLICATIONS OF PERCEIVED PARENTING

Oxytocin system

Direct examination for the associations between OT and perceived parenting experience has been the focus of OT studies in adults and children.[12,13,31,32,33] In one study, attachment representations with own mothers were assessed in those who were securely (n = 15) and insecurely attached (n = 14). Plasma OT was examined before (baseline) and after play interaction with their 4-month-old infants and it was found that although the baseline plasma OT did not differ, secure mothers had higher post-interaction OT levels than insecure mothers.[13]

Two studies have shown that the impact of perceived parenting on an individual’s OT profile may also reflect the role of OT in stress regulation.[12,31] Feldman et al.’s[31] study comprised 71 mothers, 41 fathers and their 4–6-month-old infants.

Interestingly, the urinary OT levels showed a trend for “negative” correlation with reported perceived care only among mothers. Elmadih et al.’s[12] study comprised a cohort of 80 mothers who were rated for maternal sensitivity following observed interaction with their infant at 4–6 months postpartum. Comparison was made between mothers who were rated as high sensitivity mothers (HSMs) and those rated as low sensitivity mothers (LSMs) (15 in each group).

Although quality of perceived parenting (assessed using PBI) did not differ between HSMs and LSMs, plasma OT levels among LSMs were positively correlated with negative perceived parenting experience at all times, but especially with “maternal overprotection.”

It is possible that adverse parenting received by these mothers created stress that made it challenging to adapt to the new maternal role and handle their own infant. Findings from these two studies support the current evidence that OT could be released as a modality for regulating social stress to moderate or even decrease stress responsiveness,[34,35] which is, in general, higher among women than men.[34,36,37,38,39,40]

Interestingly, the relationship between OT and quality of upbringing is not only apparent among parents but may also appear in nonparents and children. In a study that included men and women who were not parents (n = 45), a positive correlation was reported between plasma OT levels and the individual’s reported parental care scores.[33]

In another study among children, Wismer Fries et al.[32] compared urinary OT levels of children who were raised in orphanages (adopted at the start of the study; n = 18) and those who were raised by their biological mothers (n = 21). Children interacted with their mothers (foster or biological) for 30 min while playing video games sitting on their lap.

They repeated the same session with an unfamiliar adult (i.e., mother of another child). Despite the absence of differences in the baseline OT between adopted and biological children, children raised by their parents showed borderline higher postinteraction urinary OT (P = 0.06), irrespective of with whom they interacted.

This suggests that OT may not always represent affiliation and it likely has a more complex role. This was highlighted in a study on mothers where maternal brain activation in response to their infant’s video was negatively correlated with their plasma OT levels.[41]

Based on the studies discussed, it can be stated that in addition to its role in parental affiliative behavior, OT is positively correlated with attachment representation to own parents and to memory about own parental care. Recent evidence suggests that the quality of perceived parenting may correlate with OT both positively and negatively, supporting a dual role of OT in the overall affiliation and stress regulation.

IMPACT OF PERCEIVED PARENTING ON MATERNAL BRAIN MORPHOLOGY AND BRAIN RESPONSES TO OWN INFANT

In both animal and human studies, it has been found that the plasticity of a maternal brain allows it to undergo numerous changes and remodeling related to previous maternal care experience to facilitate the adaptation to environment while raising offspring.[26,42] For example, in rodents, offspring of mothers who show high LG were found to have increased glucocorticoid receptor expression in the hippocampus, which enhanced their learning and memory ability.[42] On the other hand, female reared by low LG mothers had lower levels of glucocorticoid receptor gene expression in the hippocampus.[43] However, similar corresponding evidence is relatively limited in human studies because of the challenges associated with conducting such studies. Nonetheless, the authors discuss neurological changes related to transmission of early perceived parenting in humans, with particular attention to morphological changes in the brain or responses shown by mothers who experienced low quality of upbringing.

Hippocampus

In humans, magnetic resonance imaging studies have reported that women who received low maternal care at family of origin had smaller hippocampal volume,[44] which is associated with reduced ability to regulate stress and emotions.[45] This is possibly because the hippocampus is known to receive OT projections and contribute to decoding emotional memories.[46] Furthermore, mothers who experienced adverse parenting may show stronger hippocampal activation in response to their infant crying, similar to that observed when individuals were shown distressing pictures.[47] Collectively, this volumetric reduction and increased activation in the hippocampus are suggestive of chronic stress in these mothers.[45]

Kim et al.[48] classified mothers as having high or low perceived maternal care using PBI (n = 13 each) and examined brain responses to their infant crying. Mothers who received high maternal care showed greater activation in frontal areas implicated in parenting, including the precentral gyrus, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, superior temporal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, fusiform gyrus and lingual gyrus. On the other hand, only the hippocampus, which is implicated in stress regulation, was found to have higher activation in mothers who received low maternal care. This suggests that mothers who received poor-quality parenting may have higher stress to their infant crying compared with mothers who received high-quality parenting. It is also possible that chronic exposure to stress may increase cortisol levels, which can reduce the maternal brain responses to infant’s cry in other areas involved in parenting.[48]

Nigrostriatal pathways

In a study by Strathearn et al.,[13] when insecurely attached mothers were shown the smiling face of their infant, there was increased activation in the brain regions implicated in executive functions and empathy, specifically the nigrostriatal pathways, which includes dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, dorsal striatum and substantia nigra of the midbrain.[49] In contrast, mothers with secure attachment had higher activation in brain regions implicated in secretion of OT and dopamine, which plays a role in reward processing, namely, the mesolimbic pathway including medial prefrontal cortex, ventral striatum and ventral tegmental area. These mothers also had activation in the hypothalamus and pituitary region, which was positively correlated with rise in their plasma OT levels.

Insula

In mothers who are insecurely attached, activation of insula has been reported in response to seeing an image of their infant’s sad face.[13] In mothers who reported parental neglect during childhood, a similar activation was observed in response to seeing their infant cry.[9] Such activation of insula suggest avoidance or rejection of negative infant cues by these mothers.

Amygdala

Mothers with insecure attachment show greater amygdala activation to their infant crying than mothers with secure attachment.[50] However, activation of amygdala has also shown to be a healthy maternal response to infant stimuli.[51] This reflects the importance of amygdala in parenting, given its complex role in emotional salience, detection of threat, processing of reward and biological valence that are required for social behavior.[52]

Gray/white matter

Mothers who received high-quality maternal care may have larger grey matter volume than their counterparts who received low-quality maternal care. In turn, this larger grey matter volume helps them understand the emotional states of their infant, and thus respond more sensitively to their infant’s signals. Interestingly, children who received institutional care and then moved to foster care setting had larger white matter volume compared with those who remained in institutional care, suggesting that a positive change in environment can improve adverse effects that result from poor upbringing.[8]

The use of neuroimaging in parenting research has improved our understanding about maternal brain, including brain function and responses related to perceived parenting experience. However, research on structural or functional changes in maternal brain with respect to early life experiences is still limited and not without contradictions.[53] For instance, activation of amygdala and insula have been reported in mothers with low-quality and high-quality perceived parenting. In addition, increase in gray matter volume through skills’ training was peculiarly associated with increase in hippocampal activation, similar to that seen in individuals with stress-related parenting experience.[54] This inconsistency points toward the need for more experimental-based studies in humans despite its challenges.


Source:
Max Planck Institute
Media Contacts:
Tobias Grossmann – Max Planck Institute
Image Source:
The image is credited to MPI CBS.

Original Research: Open access
“Epigenetic dynamics in infancy and the impact of maternal engagement”. Kathleen M. Krol, Robert G. Moulder, Travis S. Lillard, Tobias Grossmann and Jessica J. Connelly.
Science Advances doi:10.1126/sciadv.aay0680.

LEAVE A REPLY

Please enter your comment!
Please enter your name here

Questo sito usa Akismet per ridurre lo spam. Scopri come i tuoi dati vengono elaborati.