The ability to speak is one of the essential characteristics that distinguishes humans from other animals. Many people would probably intuitively equate speech and language.
However, cognitive science research on sign languages since the 1960s paints a different picture: Today it is clear, sign languages are fully autonomous languages and have a complex organization on several linguistic levels such as grammar and meaning.
Previous studies on the processing of sign language in the human brain had already found some similarities and also differences between sign languages and spoken languages. Until now, however, it has been difficult to derive a consistent picture of how both forms of language are processed in the brain.
Researchers at the MPI CBS now wanted to know which brain regions are actually involved in the processing of sign language across different studies – and how large the overlap is with brain regions that hearing people use for spoken language processing.
In a meta-study recently published in the journal Human Brain Mapping, they pooled data from sign language processing experiments conducted around the world.
“A meta-study gives us the opportunity to get an overall picture of the neural basis of sign language. So, for the first time, we were able to statistically and robustly identify the brain regions that were involved in sign language processing across all studies,” explains Emiliano Zaccarella, last author of the paper and group leader in the Department of Neuropsychology at the MPI CBS.
The researchers found that especially the so-called Broca’s area in the frontal brain of the left hemisphere is one of the regions that was involved in the processing of sign language in almost every study evaluated. This brain region has long been known to play a central role in spoken language, where it is used for grammar and meaning.
In order to better classify their results from the current meta-study, the scientists compared their findings with a database containing several thousand studies with brain scans.
The Leipzig-based researchers were indeed able to confirm that there is an overlap between spoken and signed language in Broca’s area. They also succeeded in showing the role played by the right frontal brain – the counterpart to Broca’s area on the left side of the brain.
This also appeared repeatedly in many of the sign language studies evaluated, because it processes non-linguistic aspects such as spatial or social information of its counterpart. This means that movements of the hands, face and body – of which signs consist – are in principle perceived similarly by deaf and hearing people.
Only in the case of deaf people, however, do they additionally activate the language network in the left hemisphere of the brain, including Broca’s area. They therefore perceive the gestures as gestures with linguistic content – instead of as pure movement sequences, as would be the case with hearing people.
The results demonstrate that Broca’s area in the left hemisphere is a central node in the language network of the human brain. Depending on whether people use language in the form of signs, sounds or writing, it works together with other networks.
Broca’s area thus processes not only spoken and written language, as has been known up to now, but also abstract linguistic information in any form of language in general.
“The brain is therefore specialized in language per se, not in speaking,” explains Patrick C. Trettenbrein, first author of the publication and doctoral student at the MPI CBS. In a follow-up study, the research team now aims to find out whether the different parts of Broca’s area are also specialized in either the meaning or the grammar of sign language in deaf people, similar to hearing people.
The human being has survived, in the Darwinian sense, through the development and, if compared to all other species, hypertrophy of language. The human central nervous system has evolved to allow for this specialization.
There is Paleolithic evidence that visual-based language occurred before auditory [1].
This hypoth- esis comes, in part, from evidence that there was linguistic communication before the voice/speech tract evolved into a form that allowed for articulated auditory communication [2]. Ontological data [3] demonstrate that the infant can utilize visual and auditory linguistic inputs equally. These, and other, streams of evidence indicate that human language is not dependent on any particular sensory input but can and does develop when there is appropriate
linguistic flux from any sensory modality. The enablement of language in the deaf relies on visual sensory inputs.
The ability of a congenitally deaf person to acquire language through a ‘‘non-traditional’’ sen- sory mode, vision, was not widely appreciated or recognized until the 16th century. The story of this belated recognition and utilization of the human central nervous system’s intrinsic character- istics of redundancy and plasticity is the subject of this paper.
Indigenous sign language: fifth century BC to sixth century AD
The deaf utilized sign language from at least the 4th century BC, as evidenced by the statements of Socrates in Plato’s Cratylus [4]:
‘‘And let me ask another question: if we had no faculty of speech, how should we communicate with one another? Should we not use signs, like the deaf and dumb? The elevation of our hands would mean lightness; heaviness would be ex- pressed by letting them drop. The running of any animal would be described by a similar movement of our own frames .. .’’
‘‘Suppose that we had no voice or tongue, and wanted to communicate with one another, should we not, like the deaf and dumb, make signs with the hands and head and the rest of the body?’’
An earlier depiction of sign language is represented by a fifth century BC Greek vase showing Philomela, whose tongue was cut by King Tereus of Thrace, using signs. Another early documentation of the use of visual communication is the Roman coin, the tessera, where fingers were used to show numbers.
Monastic signs and the deaf from the sixth to 16th centuries
The earliest known description of a hand alphabet is that of Saint Bede (fifth to sixth centuries) who developed or described a system for visual communication that was used by religious commu- nities that had taken a vow of silence [5]. Saint Bonaventure (13th century), as noted in Melchor de Yedra’s 1593 Refugium infirmorum , had developed a finger alphabet [6]. Hand signs were widely used, especially in the Benedictine communities.
The promulgation and maintenance of this form of communication were probably the basis of the application of manual language to the deaf in the 16th century.
During the next centuries, there are occasional references to the use of non-verbal manual communication by the deaf. One of these was the recognition of the deaf’s right to the assent of marriage by the Spanish King Alfonso X (13th century) [7]:
‘‘Signs that demonstrate consent among the mute do as much as words among those who speak.’’
Another reference is that of Rodolfo Phrisii Agricolae (15th century), who in his 1521 De inventiuone dialectica noted a person ‘‘ ‘deaf from the cradle and by consequence mute’ who could express his thoughts and understand those of others by way of writing’’ [7].
Application of signs for aid in teaching the deaf: 16th to 17th centuries
Up to the 17th century, the Aristotelian concept that hearing conveyed sound, which was assumed to be the basis of thought and by inference language, was the tenet by which the deaf, usually those with congenital or early-onset deafness, who could not speak, were considered uneducable. The religious canons were congruent with this concept of an inability to think or use language, as written in the Talmud [8]:
‘‘A deaf-mute is not a responsible person, and, like a minor and an imbecile, he cannot acquire property, but ‘for practical reasons’ the Rabbis laid it down that to deprive them of anything they possess is robbery.’’
The Catholic Church continued this attitude, which had been amplified by the writing of Saint Augustine, quoting the Apostle Paul [7]:
‘‘… born … deaf, which defect, indeed, hinders faith itself, by the witness of the Apostle, who says, ‘Faith comes by hearing’.. .’’
The outcome of these concepts was that these deaf non-verbal people were considered unable to learn or achieve salvation.
The utilization and promulgation of vision as a basis and curriculum for deaf communication begins in the Benedictine Monastery of San Salvador at On˜a in Burgos, Spain.
The 16th century Benedic- tine, Pedro Ponce de Leon, undertook the education of two brothers, Francisco and Pedro de Velasco, the children of the lord of On˜a and the nephew of the constable of Castile. Typical of the time, their parents, Juan de Velasco and Juana Enriquez de Rivera, were consanguineous so as to preserve wealth.
They had nine children, four of whom were deaf. The two deaf sisters, Juliana and Bernar- dina, entered a convent. The brothers probably entered the monastery of San Salvador at On˜a in 1547 -/48. De Leo
n, the Benedictine, would have had knowl- edge of >/360 different signs used for communica- tion during periods of silence [9].
These signs were used to describe the activities of daily life, such as ‘‘… eating utensils, objects used in the mass, gar- ments, food, and tools’’, as well as ‘‘… emotional states, dignitaries of the monastery .. .’’ [7]. It has been suggested [7] that de Leo`n also utilized the indigenous semantic ‘‘home’’ signs [10] that these brothers had acquired when living with their two deaf sisters. A motivation for this may be found in the will of their father, Juan de Velasco, in which he petitioned the Emperor and was granted the right to allow his deaf sons to inherit his estates if his eldest hearing son should predecease them.
This unusual provision would have been more sustainable if the deaf sons could communicate. The method of teaching involved use of the written word, a manual alphabet and the use of both the Benedictine signs and those indigenous signs that the children had developed.
The education was successful, as attested by the ability of both boys to speak and to read and write in Spanish, Greek and Latin.
De Leo`n educated other deaf children of the Spanish nobility, including at least one of the sisters of Francisco and Pedro, and recorded his method in a manuscript entitled Doctrina para los mudos sordo. This was known to exist up to 1821, after which it and its copies were lost or destroyed, although there may be one page remaining [11].
Systematic application and diffusion of sign language as an aid in the teaching of the deaf: 17th century
During the years after the death of de Leon, deaf education, limited to the aristocracy, migrated from the monastery to the home. Amongst these teachers of the deaf was Manuel Ram´ırez de Carrio´n, the tutor of the congenitally deaf Alonso Ferna´ndez de Co´rdoba y Figueroa, who achieved a high level of success. Carrio´n, in 1615, was called to Madrid to teach the son of the deceased sixth Captain of Castile, Luis Ferna´ndez de Velasco, the grand nephew of de Leo
n’s two original pupils, Francisco and Pedro. Luis’s mother, Juana de Co´rdoba, Duchess of Frias, needed to have her son educated to take communion so that he could be a ‘‘legal’’ person and thus she could be regent until he came of age.
Thus great resource was invested in enabling him to communicate. Carrio´n’s educational techni- ques included the use of finger spelling. He re- mained Luis’s tutor for 4 years until he was called back to Alonso Ferna´ndez.
The new tutor was Jan Pablo Bonet (1579-/1633), born near Zaragoza, who began his career as a mercenary and entered the service of de Velasco as a translator. He acquired his knowledge of deaf instruction by rooming with and observing the secretive Carrio´n. In 1620 he published the first book concerning the education of the deaf Reduction de las letras y arte para ensen˜ar a ablar los mudos [12].
Bonet did not continue as a teacher but went on to become a state official. His book was of great importance as it was the first to widely disseminate the techniques of teaching the deaf by means of finger spelling, while deprecating the use of signs [7]:‘‘In any home where there are mutes .. . it is not well that those who talk to him use signs, nor that they permit him to make use of them.’’
There was, however, mention of the use of somewhat arbitrary signs, those for which the meaning is derived from agreement and not by resemblance to the word. One such use of this form of ‘‘genuine’’ sign was to explain verb tense [7]:
‘‘For ‘past’ the hand moved back over the shoulder, and for ‘future’ the hand arched forward in front of the body.’’
Sir Kenelm Digby, the English ambassador to Spain, met the 13-year-old Luis and was amazed by the talent of this young person who could read and write not only in Spanish but also in Latin, Greek and other languages. Twenty years later, in 1644, Digby described Luis in his book entitled Two Treatises: In one of which, the Nature of Bodies in the other, the Nature of Mans Soule, is looked into: In a way of discovery of the Immortality of Reasonable Soules as an extraordinary deaf person who was highly edu- cated, adept at lip reading and in all ways capable. Digby’s book, which was reprinted, translated into German and widely disseminated, did much to inform the world of the possibility of educating the deaf.
The codification of sign language syntax, formation of a curriculum and open access: 18th and 19th centuries
The recognition of sign language as a complete language and the design and implementation of a
curriculum to teach this language begins with Charles Michel de L’E´ pe´e. L’E´ pe´e, a Jansenist, was banned from preaching but found his vocation by chance when he met two deaf girls who were being taught through pictures. He felt that faith and salvation should not be dependent on hearing and could be achieved through signs.
Using his father’s house and his own funds, L’E´ pe´e established, in 1771, the first free school for the deaf. His first publication appeared in 1774 [13]. He established and published syntax for sign language.
The successor to L’E´ pe´e, in 1790, was the priest Roch-Ambroise Sicard, who had come to Paris to learn from L’E´ pe´e and then established a school for the deaf in Bordeaux. The Parisian school for the deaf and de L’E´ pe´e, major humane triumphs for France, were highly regarded by an aristocracy, soon to be eclipsed, who were empa- thetic with the education of these unfortunates. The National Assembly passed a law establishing the school for the deaf on 29 July, 1791 [14].
This was dedicated to L’E´ pe´e and was sanctioned by Louis
XVI, at that time a constitutional monarch and a virtual prisoner in the Tuilleries, a month following his arrest at Varennes. This was the first state-sponsored school for the deaf and was open to all. It was one of the last acts of Louis XVI, soon to be called ‘‘King of the French’’, and then simply ‘‘Louis Capet’’.
Sicard continued the work of de L’E´ pe´e during the
early years of the French Revolution. Although politically conservative, Sicard was able to persuade the National Assembly that aid for the handicapped was part of the ‘‘natural duties’’ encompassed by the ‘‘rights of man’’. This was the foundation for our system of the care and education of all children, the basis for the concept of ‘‘The least restrictive educational pathway’’ [15].
Controversy: 19th to 20th centuries
During the 19th century, additional educational systems developed based philosophically on the premise that the deaf need to communicate orally and that the use of sign language would interfere with the development of oral language; sign language was the path of least resistance and the deaf child would not be motivated to learn to speak. Until the 1880s both educational systems, sign and oralism, coexisted.
At the International Congress of Teachers of the Deaf held in Milan in 1880, dominated by oralists, a number of resolutions were passed. The ones with the most far-reaching consequence are as follows [17]:
‘‘I. The Congress, considering the incontestable superiority of speech over signs in restoring the deaf-mute to society, and giving him a more perfect knowledge, declares that the oral method ought to be preferred to that of signs for the education and instruction of the deaf and dumb.
II. The Congress, considering that the simulta- neous use of speech and signs has the disadvantage of injuring speech, lip reading and the precision of ideas, declares that the Pure Oral Method ought to be preferred.’’
Controversy and acrimony characterized the means of linguistic communication and the educa- tion of the deaf for most of the 20th century. Studies [16] have shown that outcomes of the totally oral educational process have been poor. Towards the middle of the 20th century there was a movement towards the incorporation of signs in combination with oral language, known as total communication. Today there is the possibility that early use of the cochlear implant may allow auditory- based language in many deaf children.
Vision alone is able to establish language. There are now tools, functional MRI, PET scans and the recording of evoked potentials, which allow the exploration of the physiological bases of language. It is no surprise that these techniques reflect the obser- vations of Socrates (‘‘of the deaf and dumb who have words without sound .. .’’) that the experiments of nature, the use of vision, for language acquisition have aided in the understanding of the biological bases of language. Language is an intrinsic property of the nervous system which is dependent on a sensory input, but not on any particular sensory input.
References
- Stokoe WC. Language in hand: why sign came before speech. Washington, D.C.: Gallaudet Press; 2001.
- Laitman JT, Heimbuch RC. The basicranium of Plio- Pleistocene hominids as an indicator of their upper respira- tory systems. Am J Phys Anthropol 1982;59:323 -/43.
- Petitto LA, Marentette PF. Babbling in the manual mode: evidence for the ontogeny of language. Science 1991;251: 1493 -/6.
- Plato, translated by Jowett B. Project Gutenberg Etexts. Cratylus. Available from: www.gutenberg.net/etext99/crtls10.tx
- Montgomery G, Dimmock A. Venerable legacy: Saint Bede and the Anglo-Celtic contribution to literary, numerical, manual language. Edinburgh, UK: Scottish Workshop Pub- lications; 1998.
- Werner H. Geschichte des Taubstummenproblems bis ins 17, Jahrhundert. Jena, Germany: Verlag von Gustav Fisher; 1932.
- Plann S. A silent minority: deaf education in Spain, 1550 -/ 1835. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press; 1997.
- Babylonian Talmud: Tractate Baba Mezi’a Baba Mezi’a 8a, Footnote 11. Available from: www.come-and-hear.com/babamezia/babamezia_8.html
- Barakat RA. Cistercian sign language. In: Umiker-Sebeok J, Sebeok TA, editors. Monastic sign language. New York: Mouton de Gruyter; 1987. p. 67 -/322.
- Goldin-Meadow S, Feldman H. The development of lan- guage-like communication without a language model. Sci- ence 1977;197:401 -/3.
- Eguilua Angoitia A. Fray Pedro Ponce de Leo´n: La nueva personalidad del sordomudo. Madrid: Instituto Profesional de Sordomudos Ponce de Leo´n; 1986.
- Bonet JP. Reduction de las letras y arte para ensen˜ar a ablar los mudos. Madrid: Abarca de Angulo; 1620.
- L’E´ pe´e C Md. Institution des sourds et muets, ou, Recueil des exercices soutenus par les sourds & muets pendant les anne´es 1771, 1772, 1773 & 1774; avec les lettres qui ont accompagne´ les programmes de chacun de ces exercices. Paris: l’Imprimerie de Butard; 1774.
- Loi Relative a` M. l’Abbe´ de l’E´ pe´e & a` son e´tablissement en faveur des Sours & Muets; 1791.
- Schama S. Citizens: a chronicle of the French Revolution. New York: Knopf; 1989.
- Rodda M, Ravenhall PM. The hearing-impaired school leaver. London: University of London; 1970.
- Bender R. The conquest of deafness. Cleveland, OH: Case Western Reserve University Press; 1970.
More information: Patrick C. Trettenbrein et al, Functional neuroanatomy of language without speech: An ALE meta‐analysis of sign language, Human Brain Mapping (2020). DOI: 10.1002/hbm.25254