With COVID-19 vaccines working and restrictions lifting across the country, it’s finally time for those now vaccinated who’ve been hunkered down at home to ditch the sweatpants and reemerge from their Netflix caves. But your brain may not be so eager to dive back into your former social life.
Social distancing measures proved essential for slowing COVID-19’s spread worldwide – preventing upward of an estimated 500 million cases. But, while necessary, 15 months away from each other has taken a toll on people’s mental health.
In a national survey last fall, 36% of adults in the U.S. – including 61% of young adults – reported feeling “serious loneliness” during the pandemic. Statistics like these suggest people would be itching to hit the social scene.
But if the idea of making small talk at a crowded happy hour sounds terrifying to you, you’re not alone. Nearly half of Americans reported feeling uneasy about returning to in-person interaction regardless of vaccination status.
So how can people be so lonely yet so nervous about refilling their social calendars?
Well, the brain is remarkably adaptable. And while we can’t know exactly what our brains have gone through over the last year, neuroscientists like me have some insight into how social isolation and resocialization affect the brain.
Social homeostasis – the need to socialize
Humans have an evolutionarily hardwired need to socialize – though it may not feel like it when deciding between a dinner invite and rewatching “Schitt’s Creek.”
From insects to primates, maintaining social networks is critical for survival in the animal kingdom. Social groups provide mating prospects, cooperative hunting and protection from predators.
But social homeostasis – the right balance of social connections – must be met. Small social networks can’t deliver those benefits, while large ones increase competition for resources and mates. Because of this, human brains developed specialized circuitry to gauge our relationships and make the correct adjustments – much like a social thermostat.
Social homeostasis involves many brain regions, and at the center is the mesocorticolimbic circuit – or “reward system.” That same circuit motivates you to eat chocolate when you crave something sweet or swipe on Tinder when you crave … well, you get it.
And like those motivations, a recent study found that reducing social interaction causes social cravings – producing brain activity patterns similar to food deprivation.
So if people hunger for social connection like they hunger for food, what happens to the brain when you starve socially?
Your brain on social isolation
Scientists can’t shove people into isolation and look inside their brains. Instead, researchers rely on lab animals to learn more about social brain wiring. Luckily, because social bonds are essential in the animal kingdom, these same brain circuits are found across species.
One prominent effect of social isolation is – you guessed it – increased anxiety and stress.
Many studies find that removing animals from their cage buddies increases anxiety-like behaviors and cortisol, the primary stress hormone. Human studies also support this, as people with small social circles have higher cortisol levels and other anxiety-related symptoms similar to socially deprived lab animals.
Evolutionarily this effect makes sense – animals that lose group protection must become hypervigilant to fend for themselves. And it doesn’t just occur in the wild. One study found that self-described “lonely” people are more vigilant of social threats like rejection or exclusion.
Another important region for social homeostasis is the hippocampus – the brain’s learning and memory center. Successful social circles require you to learn social behaviors – such as selflessness and cooperation – and recognize friends from foes. But your brain stores tremendous amounts of information and must remove unimportant connections. So, like most of your high school Spanish – if you don’t use it, you lose it.
Several animal studies show that even temporary adulthood isolation impairs both social memory – like recognizing a familiar face – and working memory – like recalling a recipe while cooking.
And isolated humans may be just as forgetful. Antarctic expeditioners had shrunken hippocampi after just 14 months of social isolation. Similarly, adults with small social circles are more likely to develop memory loss and cognitive decline later in life.
So, human beings might not be roaming the wild anymore, but social homeostasis is still critical to survival. Luckily, as adaptable as the brain is to isolation, the same may be true with resocialization.
Your brain on social reconnection
Though only a few studies have explored the reversibility of the anxiety and stress associated with isolation, they suggest that resocialization repairs these effects.
One study, for example, found that formerly isolated marmosets first had higher stress and cortisol levels when resocialized but then quickly recovered. Adorably, the once-isolated animals even spent more time grooming their new buddies.
Social memory and cognitive function also seem to be highly adaptable.
Mouse and rat studies report that while animals cannot recognize a familiar friend immediately after short-term isolation, they quickly regain their memory after resocializing.
And there may be hope for people emerging from socially distanced lockdown as well. A recent Scottish study conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic found that residents had some cognitive decline during the harshest lockdown weeks but quickly recovered once restrictions eased.
Unfortunately, studies like these are still sparse. And while animal research is informative, it likely represents extreme scenarios since people weren’t in total isolation over the last year. Unlike mice stuck in cages, many in the U.S. had virtual game nights and Zoom birthday parties (lucky us).
So power through the nervous elevator chats and pesky brain fog, because “un-social distancing” should reset your social homeostasis very soon.
Social homeostasis: a widespread phenomenon
Homeostasis classically refers to physiological processes wherein stable states are maintained through compensatory mechanisms.18 Homeostatic systems are known to exist for a number of physiological needs essential to survival such as thermoregulation, energy balance, and osmoregulation.
These rely upon detection of a deviation from a defined homeostatic “set point,” followed by central coordination of a response in a “control center,” and the recruitment of “effector systems” that interact with the environment to correct the deviation (Fig. 1).
Challenges to physiological homeostasis can also elicit motivated behaviors associated with strong negative “drive” states, such as overheating, thirst, and hunger, designed to appropriately adapt/direct behavior.19-21
While a change in social connection may not appear to constitute an immediate challenge to internal stability, individuals on the social perimeter are vulnerable and becoming isolated can threaten survival. Even in controlled laboratory environments (where external threats to survival are absent), the presence of social contact is associated with increased life span across a range of social species including honeybees, ants, Drosophila melanogaster,22, 23 mice,24, 25 and rats,26-28 as well as in free-ranging groups of macaques29 and baboons.30
Therefore, an emerging social neuroscience model posits that evolutionarily conserved neurophysiological mechanisms underlie the adaptive, short-term, self-preservation mode triggered by a lack of social connections/mutual protection.14, 31 This model proposes that loneliness operates as an aversive signal designed to promote adaptation to the vulnerabilities of being alone and motivate reconnection.32
Thus, the long-term disease states perpetuated by chronic loneliness may result from the prolonged engagement of neural systems that were intended for short-term preservation.
To begin unraveling how the chronic state of loneliness emerges, it is necessary to first understand the neural response to social deficit. Conceptualizing this as the response of a homeostatic system would apply certain defined principles (Fig. 1). A social homeostatic system would be required to (1) monitor social conditions; (2) detect deviation from a homeostatic “set point” in control centers; and (3) activate effector systems to elicit an appropriate response (e.g., strategies to promote social contact). A deficit in social connections (whether perceived or actual) would be predicted to engage this system.
In animals, one way to create a social deficit is to remove social contact entirely. While this only captures the objective component of social isolation, it offers controlled conditions for assessing rapid neurophysiological adaptations. Chronic social isolation, particularly in rodents, has been used as a developmental model of early life stress since many of the long-term maladaptive changes resemble features of human neuropsychiatric disease.33 This rich body of work has been comprehensively reviewed elsewhere for both rodents33-37 and nonhuman primates.38-40
Alternatively, here we examine the response to acute social isolation (using under 1 week as an arbitrary operational definition of “acute” for the purpose of the review) in order to identify candidate neural circuits involved in the rapid response to social deficit. We focus primarily on experiments in social rodents, including laboratory mice (Mus musculus), rats (Rattus norvegicus), and prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster), which are social species, adapted to group living, but with different styles of social behavior.
The wild species of mice and rats from which laboratory strains were derived are promiscuous and territorial, but show greater social tolerance in high-density living environments and adopt linear dominance hierarchies that promote group stability.41 In a laboratory setting, mice and rats prefer social company (even that of other males) over a solitary existence.42, 43
They show conditioned preference for regions previously associated with social contact,44 make nests in close proximity to conspecifics when partially separated,43, 45 and will actively work to obtain social contact.46, 47 Alternatively, prairie voles are socially monogamous and form an enduring, selective bond with their partner following mating. They show biparental care toward offspring, tend to live in extended families,48, 49 and are well utilized in the study of social bonding and isolation.
Here, we evaluate social isolation–induced adaptations in these rodents, in light of the phenotype of human loneliness, which may also represent a state of activation of “social homeostatic systems.”
We have categorized the behavioral and neural adaptations into three broad themes: (1) hypervigilance/arousal; (2) social motivation; and (3) passive coping. We propose that parallel, overlapping circuits mediate the response to social deficit (the output of homeostatic “effector” systems) in an effort to heighten attention to environmental stimuli, motivate social reconnection, and limit emotional distress (Fig. 2).
While we can only speculate as to the neural identity of the detector, control, and effector systems in a social homeostatic network, we anticipate that a cohesive understanding of the response to social deficit will help unmask candidate neural substrates.
Homeostatic response to social deficit: promoting hypervigilance
An evolutionary perspective on the origins of loneliness proposes that the vulnerabilities of isolation promote hypervigilance to guard against potential threats.50 Lonely individuals often show high levels of anxiety,51, 52 and hypervigilant responses to negative social stimuli, suggesting heightened recruitment of attentional and self-preservation mechanisms.53 In rodents, acute isolation can promote behaviors that indicate enhanced arousal and heightened vigilance.
For example, adult rats show an increase in escape-related behaviors over 1–7 days of isolation,54 along with a reduction in exploratory behavior and an increase in self-grooming.55-57 Targeted manipulations in rodents have unveiled that anxiety-related behaviors arise from activity across distributed, interconnected corticolimbic circuitry, which interpret and evaluate incoming environmental stimuli (reviewed in Ref. 58).
One major output system is the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical (HPA) axis, which regulates arousal, vigilance, and attention, in concert with central arousal circuits including the lateral hypothalamic (LH) orexin/hypocretin system, locus coeruleus (LC) noradrenergic neurons, basal forebrain cholinergic neurons, dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN) serotonergic neurons, and midbrain dopaminergic neurons.59, 60 Several of these neural circuits exhibit rapid adaptations following acute social isolation. Here, we briefly outline the nature of these changes and their potential role in the response of a social homeostatic system.
HPA axis
Glucocorticoid production is initiated by paraventricular hypothalamic nucleus (PVN) secretion of corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) into the hypophyseal portal system, triggering adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) release by the anterior pituitary that in turn acts on the adrenal cortex to secrete glucocorticoids. The HPA axis is regulated by a negative feedback loop, wherein glucocorticoids bind to receptors in the pituitary and other brain regions including the hippocampus, which subsequently inhibits CRF and ACTH production.
While acute activation of the HPA axis can be an adaptive physiological response to salient events, chronic activation of this system, particularly by continued psychosocial stressors, is implicated in the progression of multiple disease states and psychopathologies.61 Consistent with this, high self-reported loneliness in humans has been associated with elevated daily cortisol output62-66 and a flattening of diurnal cortisol rhythm,65 suggesting poor regulation of the HPA axis.67
Heightened HPA axis activity (evidenced by a robust increase in circulating corticosterone and ACTH) is observed after 1–5 days of social isolation in juvenile (3–5 weeks old)68, 69 or pair-bonded adult70-72 prairie voles. Peripheral corticosterone levels are also reportedly increased in male mice isolated for 12 h,73 and both pituitary ACTH and adrenal corticosterone are increased in male rats isolated for 24 h in a novel environment.74, 75 This recruitment of the HPA axis during acute periods of isolation may reflect the increased need for vigilance and attention to salient stimuli.
CRF signaling
CRF pathways are a prominent point of convergence for isolation-induced adaptations. Aside from their role in initiating the neuroendocrine response to stress, PVN CRF neurons are pivotal in orchestrating the rapid, complex behavioral adaptations that occur following acute stress (potentially via glutamate coreleasing projections to neighboring hypothalamic regions).76 CRF-producing neurons are also widely distributed in extrahypothalamic regions, including the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), central amygdala (CeA), nucleus accumbens (NAc), and hippocampus,77-79 which have, likewise, been implicated in the behavioral and physiological responses to stress.
Isolation of preadolescent female (but not male) mice for <24 h decreased the excitability of PVN CRF neurons in a glucocorticoid-dependent manner.80 This finding may reflect glucocorticoid feedback–induced suppression of CRF activity. Consistent with this interpretation, a 24-h isolation of adult rats decreased CRF mRNA and protein in the PVN75 and decreased cortical CRF1 receptor levels.81 Moreover, these changes in CRF were accompanied by enhanced angiotensin II (ATII) AT1 receptor expression in the PVN.74 ATII is a circulating endocrine factor that can trigger CRF production in response to stress.82 This factor may be necessary for isolation-induced adaptations within the hypothalamic CRF system, as the isolation-induced decrease in CRF mRNA in male rats could be prevented by an AT1 receptor antagonist.75 Conversely, a shorter period (1 h) of social isolation in adult male and female prairie voles housed with same-sex siblings, resulted in increased hypothalamic and hippocampal CRF mRNA.83 This discrepancy may reflect the shorter duration of isolation or the different species under study. However, it highlights the growing need for a thorough understanding of the timeline of adaptations following social isolation.
LC noradrenergic system
The LC is the sole source of noradrenergic innervation to the central nervous system, best known for its role in arousal and vigilance, but more broadly thought to be recruited to combat environmental challenges.84, 85 In adult rats, a 24-h isolation increased tyrosine hydroxylase (TH; the rate-limiting enzyme in catecholamine synthesis) mRNA in the LC, an effect that could be blocked by an AT1 receptor antagonist.81 Thus, the acute response to isolation involves coordination across both peripheral and central neuromodulatory systems.
Homeostatic response to social deficit: engaging social motivational systems
In humans, a deficit in social connections is conceptualized to engage the “social monitoring system”86 with the purpose of directing attention toward socially relevant information. Accordingly, individuals that either self-identified as lonely or expected a lonely future showed enhanced sensitivity to social cues and increased socially affiliative motivation.86-90 Enhanced social motivation is similarly evident in acutely isolated rodents: when given the opportunity, previously isolated (2- to 48-h duration) juvenile and adult rodents spend more time engaged in social behaviors.91-98 It is suggested that up to 7 days of isolation promotes affiliative social behavior and social interest in rats,57, 92 whereas in adult mice, a significant increase in aggressive behavior was observed after 48 h, but not 24 h of social isolation.99
For many social species, the inherently rewarding nature of social interactions is a major driving force for social contact. In rodents, one method to evaluate the positive reinforcing properties of social interaction is the social conditioned place preference (social CPP) assay—an adaptation of a test traditionally employed to measure the rewarding properties of drugs of abuse.44 In this task, animals typically demonstrate preference for a place previously paired with social housing over one paired with isolate housing (∼24-h duration44). Notably, therefore, the conditioned approach to a socially conditioned context may be a product of both “social reward” and “isolation aversion.”44 Several neuromodulatory systems (including dopamine, oxytocin, and opioid circuits) are posited to underlie the motivation for social reward. These circuits are also prominent sites of rapid adaptation following social isolation, which we discuss below. The degree to which neural circuits for “social reward” and “isolation aversion” overlap and diverge remains to be determined.
Ventral tegmental area dopamine system
The midbrain dopamine system has a long-standing role in reward processing100 and affiliative social behavior,101 and is frequently reported as a site of isolation-induced adaptation. The ventral tegmental area (VTA) dopamine neurons project to multiple regions including the striatum, prefrontal cortex (PFC), and basolateral amygdala (BLA), with the VTA–NAc pathway being particularly well associated with social reward.102, 103 In juvenile rats, isolation-induced social play was suppressed by D1- or D2-receptor blockade in the NAc,104 while in adult rats, 24 h or 4 days of isolation decreased striatal D2-receptor density105 and increased mesostriatal TH activity,106 respectively.
Isolation-induced changes do not appear to be limited to the mesostriatal pathway, however, as adolescent mice isolated for 1–7 days showed an increase in cortical dopamine metabolism.107 Additionally, in the PFC, decreased GABAA-stimulated chloride influx was evident in a membrane preparation from 24-h isolated rats108 along with reduced benzodiazepine binding,81 indicating a decrease in cortical GABAA expression and/or function. Given the functional diversity of dopamine input to striatal subregions109, 110 and dopamine’s divergent effects on cortical projector populations,111 further work is necessary to elucidate precisely how these rapid isolation-induced changes to dopamine neurotransmission influence downstream activity.
DRN dopamine system
Another component of the midbrain dopamine system—the DRN dopamine neurons—also exhibits acute isolation-induced adaptations. These dopamine neurons were historically considered a caudal extension of the VTA, but accumulating evidence has revealed distinct downstream projections and functional roles.112-118 In adult male mice, 24-h social isolation potentiated glutamatergic synapses onto DRN dopamine neurons, and also heightened their activity in vivo in response to a novel mouse.118
Artificially enhancing activity of DRN dopamine neurons with optogenetic stimulation was sufficient to increase social preference. However, in the absence of a social stimulus, mice chose to avoid receiving stimulation of DRN dopamine neurons (demonstrated by real-time and conditioned place avoidance), which suggests the induction of a negative affective state.118 DRN dopamine neurons may, therefore, be recruited following acute isolation to elicit “negative drive”–induced social motivation, in a manner distinct from the reward-related social motivation mediated by the VTA–NAc dopaminergic pathway.103 Consistent with this assertion, optogenetic inhibition of the DRN dopamine population had no effect on sociability in group-housed animals, but it suppressed social preference following 24 h of isolation.118
The DRN dopamine neurons lie directly upstream of several regions, most notably the BNST and CeA.116, 118 While the explicit role of dopamine in these regions in social behavior remains to be determined, dopamine receptor signaling can modulate synaptic transmission and activity in both the BNST and CeA.115, 118-120 Specifically, in the dorsolateral BNST blunting of long-term potentiation (LTP) is evident after 24 h of social isolation in male mice.121 Given that dopamine in the BNST can facilitate glutamatergic transmission, via a CRF-dependent process,119 it is tempting to speculate that increased dopamine neurotransmission following acute isolation may occlude LTP.
Interestingly, it was recently revealed that an intermediate duration of isolation in mice (2 weeks) is associated with upregulation of the neuropeptide tachykinin 2 (TAC2; also known as neurokinin B) in several regions including the anterodorsal BNST (adBNST), CeA, and dorsomedial hypothalamus (DMH), with levels gradually increasing from just 30 min post-isolation.122 Behavioral changes observed following 2 weeks of isolation appeared to be mediated by TAC2 upregulation in discrete sites, as chemogenetic silencing of TAC2-expressing neurons in the adBNST, CeA, or DMH selectively prevented persistent freezing, acute freezing, or aggression, respectively.122
Notably, ∼50% of TAC2-expressing neurons in the adBNST and CeA co-expressed CRF,122 which again highlights the involvement of CRF circuits in isolation-induced adaptation. A substantial body of work supports a role for the BNST in mediating sustained responses, and the CeA in mediating rapid responses, to potential/unpredictable threats.123 This behavioral control is enabled by the far-reaching connections of the BNST and CeA, particularly with hypothalamic and brainstem structures, which underlies their ability to influence autonomic and neuroendocrine functions.124, 125 These regions are therefore well positioned to drive isolation-induced adaptive responses, under modulatory control from upstream regions, including the DRN dopamine neurons.
This collection of findings compels the hypothesis that dopaminergic signaling may be involved in the initial response to social isolation, but that downstream regions (including the BNST and CeA) might exhibit longer term remodeling/plasticity following chronic isolation. Indeed, there is considerable evidence to support a similar model for the stages of drug-evoked plasticity in the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system.
Specifically, a single dose of cocaine is sufficient to potentiate glutamatergic transmission onto VTA dopamine neurons after 24 hours.126 Synaptic strength returns to baseline levels within a week, however, this VTA plasticity is required for the persistent changes that occur downstream in the NAc following prolonged cocaine exposure127 (reviewed in Ref. 128). This permissive role of synaptic plasticity in VTA dopamine neurons could similarly be a feature of DRN dopamine neurons in the response to social isolation. Such a feature would predict that acute isolation-induced synaptic changes in DRN dopamine neurons precede, and are necessary for, chronic isolation-induced adaptations in downstream regions. In this way, the myriad of maladaptive behavioral changes associated with long-term social isolation37 might result from chronic engagement of neural circuits mediating the acute response to social isolation and persistent remodeling in downstream regions.
Opioid system
The opioid system exerts a broad influence on neural activity through widespread expression of opioid peptides and receptors, most notably within regions connected to positive reinforcement (reviewed in Ref. 129). Opioid signaling plays well-documented roles in regulating pain/analgesia,130 reward processing,129 and social bonding,131 and has also been implicated in isolation-induced social behavior. In vivo autoradiography revealed changes to opioid receptor binding, with 7 days of isolation in juvenile rats associated with upregulation of opioid receptor number or affinity in the PFC.57
Additionally, isolation-induced social play in juvenile rats was attenuated by systemic administration of a μ-opioid receptor (MOR) antagonist97, 132 or a ĸ-opioid receptor (KOR) agonist,132 but enhanced by administration of a MOR agonist.97 Furthermore, in the CeA, infusion of an ACTH analog suppressed isolation-enhanced social interest in 7-day isolated rats, but this was prevented by administration of naltrexone (a MOR and KOR antagonist).56, 133 Therefore, both opioid and dopamine receptor signaling may be necessary for the heightened sociability evoked by acute isolation.
Hypothalamic oxytocin system
Oxytocin-producing neurons of the PVN, along with the closely related vasopressin (AVP) neurons, are intimately involved in the regulation of social affiliation134 and have been particularly well studied in the monogamous prairie vole, as they play a pivotal role in pair bonding.135 Oxytocin neurons project not only to the posterior pituitary where they release oxytocin into the bloodstream but also to distinct targets within the brain for direct modulation of neuronal activity.
One important site for oxytocin action is the NAc, which is a critical hub for the integration of motivationally relevant information and relays information to elicit motor responses.136 In male prairie voles, 3 days of isolation from a bonded female partner, but not a male sibling, decreased oxytocin mRNA in the PVN and oxytocin receptor binding in the NAc shell.137 Notably, oxytocin signaling in the NAc is reportedly essential for the expression of social CPP in adult male mice.138
Specifically, it was elegantly demonstrated that social CPP required activation of oxytocin receptors on presynaptic terminals in the NAc arising from DRN serotonergic neurons—facilitating serotonin release.138 Social CPP was further shown to be dependent on the PVN oxytocin projection to the VTA.139 Either suppressing activity in the PVN–VTA pathway or VTA dopamine–specific knockout of oxytocin receptors prevented social CPP in male mice.139 Collectively, these findings illustrate the dynamic balancing of dopamine, oxytocin, and serotonin signaling that is required for the reinforcing properties of social interactions.
Acute isolation has also been demonstrated to disrupt social recognition memory.140 Rodents possess an innate tendency to investigate novel rather than familiar social stimuli and typically reduce their investigation of a familiar conspecific on repeated exposure.141 This effect is absent in mice isolated for 24 h or 7 days, who display equivalent investigation of a familiar juvenile compared with the first exposure.142, 143 This lack of social recognition memory is associated with a suppression of oxytocin-dependent synaptic plasticity in the medial amygdala (MeA) following 7 days of isolation in rats.144 Impaired social recognition memory may also result from elevated hippocampal Rac1 (a small GTPase), which is evident in male mice isolated for 24 h or 7 days.145 Thus, isolation-induced modifications to oxytocin signaling, in a pathway-specific manner, may contribute to changes in both social motivation and social recognition memory.
Homeostatic response to social deficit: a self-protective coping strategy?
Individuals that self-identify as lonely frequently exhibit features of negative affective state or depression.10, 50, 146-149 Furthermore, individuals swayed toward feelings of future loneliness (by receipt of false feedback following a questionnaire) show a reduction in physical pain sensitivity and emotional sensitivity.89 This suggests the adoption of self-protective strategies to minimize further emotional distress. While we cannot directly measure emotional state in rodents, we can assay motivated behavior as a proxy.58, 150
In rodents, immobility in the forced swim and tail suspension tests is thought to reflect passive coping and/or behavioral despair151, 152 (but see Ref. 153). There is a general lack of agreement over whether acute isolation alters immobility in these assays in mice.55, 73, 154 However, more consistent results have been obtained in monogamous prairie voles, wherein females or males isolated from their bonded partner for 3–5 days show an increase in immobility time.70, 71, 137
There is also evidence for disruption of reward-related behavior in acutely isolated rodents, specifically in the response to addictive drugs. In rats, a 24-h social isolation increased preference for ethanol and opioid intake, which was reversed with social housing.155, 156 Reduced pain sensitivity has also been reported, with male mice and juvenile rats exhibiting higher thermal and mechanical pain thresholds following 2–7 days of social isolation.157, 158
The prominent role of dopamine and opioid signaling in mediating the effects of drugs of abuse159 and analgesia130, 160 makes them strong contenders for underlying these adaptations. In particular, chemogenetic activation of ventrolateral periaqueductal gray (vlPAG)/DRN dopamine neurons can promote antinociception,117 while lesion of these neurons suppresses both the antinociceptive161, 162 and rewarding163 properties of exogenous opioids. Furthermore, inhibition of VTA dopamine signaling in mice can induce depression-related behaviors,164 while KOR antagonists are proposed to have antidepressive effects in rodents (see Refs. 165-167). In this way, interaction between the dopamine and opioid systems may underlie isolation-induced changes to reward processing, pain sensitivity, and emotional affect.
In addition to dopaminergic and opioidergic mechanisms, isolation-induced depressive-like behavior may result from changes in the balance of CRF and oxytocin neurotransmission. Specifically, the passive coping behavior observed in pair-bonded prairie voles isolated for 3 days was prevented by NAc shell infusion of a CRF2 receptor antagonist, or oxytocin, throughout the period of isolation.137 Microdialysis experiments suggest a mechanism by which this effect is mediated via presynaptic CRF2 receptor activation on oxytocin terminals in the NAc, which serves to reduce oxytocin release.137 These findings point toward a confluence of isolation-induced adaptations in the NAc. The NAc receives strong glutamatergic input from thalamic and cortical regions, enabling it to integrate motivationally relevant information from neuromodulatory nuclei with higher cognitive and sensory input.168 Thus, this region is aptly poised to adapt goal-directed behavior in response to social deficit.
Proposed attributes of components within a social homeostatic system
Flexibility
Animals are frequently faced with conflicting signals in the environment, which can elicit competing motivational drives. To ensure survival, animals must appropriately weigh environmental cues and evaluate them in light of current homeostatic need state. Selecting the appropriate behavioral response under these conditions requires dynamic coordination of neural activity.169 Thus, a key requirement for a social homeostatic system is its capacity for flexibility. Specifically, a change in environmental conditions and/or need state (e.g., hunger and thirst) may require a shift in the “set point” for social contact in the control center (Figs. 1B and 3). This will be heavily influenced by dynamic factors, such as resource availability, predator threat, mating prospects, and the presence of offspring.
or example, in a state of hypernatremia (elevated plasma sodium, which is associated with the perception of thirst), rats exhibit greater social investigation of a novel intruder.170 This effect is proposed to have evolved to suppress anxiety in social situations, such as those encountered at a communal source of water, in order to promote social approach and allow drinking behavior.170 This observation suggests that social motivation can be altered by other physiological needs.
Intriguingly, acute hypernatremia in rats is associated with increased plasma oxytocin, increased c-Fos expression in magnocellular PVN oxytocin neurons, and suppression of ATII production.170 Given that ATII signaling can drive HPA axis activity, these data suggest that hypernatremia concomitantly suppresses activity within stress-related circuitry, while promoting activity in social reward-related oxytocin pathways, thereby inhibiting stress/anxiety-related behavior and facilitating social interaction. This relationship illustrates overlap and coordination across homeostatic systems and also demonstrates the flexibility of effector systems.
The motivational state of hunger is another essential homeostatic drive that promotes rapid neural adaptations.171, 172 Specifically, the agouti-related protein (AgRP) neurons in the arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus are essential in the maintenance of energy balance, and increasing their activity can rapidly drive feeding behavior.173
Interestingly, in isolated mice, optical stimulation of AgRP neurons, or physiological hunger, provoked feeding, even in the presence of a novel male or female mouse.171 This finding suggests that AgRP activity in a state of hunger is sufficient to override a competing social motivational drive. Conceptually, it is possible that competing homeostatic drives are integrated into a social homeostatic system at the level of the control center in a “hub-and-spoke” fashion, or they may form an interconnected hierarchical arrangement that converges on effector systems (Fig. 1B).
While it is possible that one homeostatic system may be subservient to another, an interconnected network would permit flexible control in a state of motivational need competition, prior to convergence on effector systems. Precisely how these homeostatic systems interface with one another remains to be elucidated.
Maintaining physiological variables, such as core body temperature and energy levels, within an appropriate dynamic range relies heavily upon a functioning immune system. Inflammation is theorized to be a unifying feature of homeostatic perturbation: providing a protective response to extreme deviations from the homeostatic set point.174, 175 Adverse conditions, including social isolation, can provoke a shift in immune function: enhancing expression of proinflammatory genes and reducing expression of antiviral/antibody-related genes in circulating leukocytes.176
Social isolation may recruit this response to appropriately prepare an individual for the susceptibilities of being alone, which might include an increased need for a rapid inflammatory response to combat bacterial infections sustained through physical injury, but reduced need for protection against socially transmitted viral infections.15, 176
This immune response also appears to be recruited in a state of perceived isolation—humans with high self-reported loneliness show enhanced proinflammatory activity but reduced antiviral response.66, 177-179 Immune system changes can also predict subsequent loneliness, suggesting a reciprocal relationship between these two phenomena.64
Remarkably, classifying rhesus macaques as putatively high in loneliness (by sociability levels and social initiation attempts)180 revealed leukocyte gene expression changes similar to those observed in lonely human subjects.64 The recruitment of inflammatory processes under conditions of actual or perceived social isolation31 suggests that this state is recognized as a threat to an essential variable. As such, this supports the assertion that social contact may be regulated in a homeostatic manner.
Mixed selectivity
Considering the neural processing of “social homeostatic” information, one possibility is the existence of dedicated neural circuitry. An alternative model would feature overlap between neural systems governing social homeostasis and other highly conserved neural circuitry. This scenario would predict that certain nodes in a social homeostatic network display “mixed selectivity,” similar to neurons underlying other complex cognitive processes.181 In particular, this is conceivably a feature of “effector” regions in a homeostatic system.
The disinhibition of VTA dopamine neurons, for example, has been shown to enhance motivation toward a variety of stimuli ranging from social stimuli to novel objects.182, 183 Furthermore, activation of BLA input to the ventral hippocampus or medial PFC (mPFC) not only induces robust anxiety-like behavior in exploratory assays but also suppresses social investigation in the resident–intruder assay.184-186 Perturbations in social behavior are often, but not always, co-expressed with anxiety-related behaviors, and there is significant overlap in their neural correlates, which suggests a tight relationship between these two forms of behavioral expression.58, 187
The DRN dopamine system is another prime example of overlapping circuit function. Monitoring fluorescent calcium activity in vivo revealed that these neurons are active in response to social stimuli, and this activity is heightened following acute isolation.118 However, these neurons are also responsive to other salient stimuli, including palatable food and unexpected foot shock, and show greater activity during wakefulness compared with sleep, suggesting an arousal-promoting function.112, 115, 188
This diversity of sensitivities is consistent with the notion that neural circuits regulating social homeostasis may promote attention to a variety of salient stimuli, in an effort to scan the environment for potential threats or opportunities for social engagement. It was also recently reported that optogenetic or chemogenetic inhibition of DRN dopamine neurons during fear conditioning suppressed freezing in response to a footshock-predictive cue, suggesting an additional role in aversive responding.115 Taken together, these findings illustrate the existence of multiple mechanisms through which DRN dopamine neurons may limit the vulnerabilities of being alone: increasing social motivation, promoting vigilance/arousal, and enhancing responsivity to aversive stimuli.
Considering the potential for mixed selectivity, the recent observation that activation of DRN dopamine neurons not only increases social preference118 but can promote antinociception117 could be reconciled in a number of ways. One possibility is that functional heterogeneity exists within this cell population. Another possibility is biological convergence in the representation of emotional pain and nociceptive pain in DRN dopamine neurons.
In accordance with this notion, an emerging hypothesis posits that social pain and physical pain are processed by overlapping neural circuitry.189 This is supported by human imaging studies revealing that social disconnection engages brain regions including the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and anterior insula cortex,190 which also process the affective component of physical pain.191 This dual role of DRN dopamine neurons also points toward a potential mechanism through which acute isolation reduces pain perception.157, 158
It is also interesting to note that inflammatory pain is suppressed by activation of AgRP neurons, suggesting a general reduction of chronic pain perception by strong motivational drive states.192 Mixed selectivity may, therefore, be a common feature within neural circuits regulating homeostatic needs, and cross talk between these systems might facilitate the activation or suppression of appropriate “effector” systems (Fig. 1).
Subjective nature of social experience
A third element in conceptualizing a social homeostatic system is the integration of subjective experience. There is mounting support for the notion that subjective or “perceived” isolation (the quality of social relationships) is a stronger predictor of poor health and emotional state in humans than objective isolation (the number/frequency of social contacts).11, 62, 146
Consistent with this, loneliness—independent of social network size—is associated with higher mortality13, 193 increased blood pressure,11, 194 higher rate of diabetes, hypertension, arthritis, emphysema,195 and Alzheimer’s disease,196 along with poor health habits stemming from a lack of self-control.197, 198 Thus, in evaluating social needs, a homeostatic system would need to incorporate a subjective assessment of social experience, in addition to its overall objective nature), which may be heavily influenced by interoceptive signals and internal state (Figs. 1 and 3).
There is an ongoing debate as to whether animals experience emotions in the same way as humans.150, 199 However, it has been reasoned that emotions constitute an internal state, encoded by specific neural circuits, which can give rise to externally observable behaviors.150 These internal brain states may be subjectively perceived as feelings by the individual.150 Although the traditional concept of homeostasis refers to a purely automatic physiological control system, motivational drive states (guided by “homeostatic feelings”) play a significant role in maintaining homeostasis.200 Homeostatic feelings act as “informative regulatory interfaces”—providing means for an animal to sense its physiological state and guaranteeing attention to relevant stimuli.200 While this process can be adaptive and introduces greater flexibility into homeostatic regulation, it also passes an element of control to the individual, taking homeostatic regulation beyond purely automatic mechanisms.200
In order to understand the neural mechanisms of social homeostasis, a major hurdle lies in the ability to infer subjective social experience in animals.14 Although we can never truly know the emotional experience of a rodent, one method of differentiating between individuals is by exploiting the natural variability introduced by social hierarchy. Grouped living can lead to the establishment of social hierarchies in multiple species including fish, birds, rodents, and primates.201-204 Hierarchies create a scenario in which grouped individuals might have divergent perceptions of their social experience. Social rank can influence access to essential resources including food, territory, and mates,205 and thus a more dominant rank is often a coveted position associated with higher quality of life. Although subordination in animal societies is not always directly related to low social connectedness or unmet social needs, social rank bestows variability in subjective social experience without removing support structure for safety, warmth and other nonsocial benefits of a group.
Strikingly, studies on social hierarchy in mice and rats have revealed underlying neural correlates in the same circuits implicated in the response to social deficit (Fig. 2). These findings include differences between subordinates and dominants in CRF expression in the BNST, CeA, MeA, and medial preoptic area;206 mitochondrial function and dopamine signaling in the NAc;207, 208 and glutamatergic synaptic strength in the mPFC.209
The mPFC, in particular, is frequently implicated in the representation of social rank. Most recently, “winning”-induced plasticity in the tube test was localized to a mediodorsal thalamic (MDT) projection to the dorsomedial PFC (dmPFC), as phasic optogenetic stimulation of the dmPFC, or the MDT–dmPFC projection, immediately induced winning against a previously dominant cagemate.210 Notably, social rank also predicted the magnitude of behavioral effects elicited upon DRN dopamine manipulations in mice.118
Optogenetic activation of these neurons promoted social preference and real-time place avoidance, whereas inhibition reduced isolation-induced social preference. However, the behavioral change observed in these assays was greater in dominant animals relative to subordinates.118 This observation suggests that prior social experience may influence the ability of the DRN dopamine neurons to modulate behavior. Collectively, these findings illustrate that rank-related information may be integrated into multiple neural circuits that respond to social deficit (Fig. 1A). This organization would permit flexible control over homeostatic regulation and adjustment of goal-directed behavior depending on the social opportunities available.
Moving forward, several questions remain in elucidating how social information might be processed through a homeostatic system. For example, is the individual’s “expectation” for social contact encoded upstream in detector regions, or at the level of the control center? And how are different categories of social contact represented? To speculate on this last point, one possibility is that a social homeostatic system is category blind.
Another potential arrangement would involve separate processing streams for the regulation of different social relationships, such as same-sex, opposite sex, mother–offspring, or unfamiliar conspecifics. Indeed, specialized circuits, within discrete hypothalamic nuclei, underlie the expression of parental behavior,211 aggression,212 male intruder–specific behavior,213 opposite-sex approach,214 and mating.212 If we conceptualize these as discrete “effector systems,” then this raises the possibility that decentralized processing of different social “needs” may occur in separable nodes.215 However, the precise organization of social homeostatic elements remains a topic of conjecture.
Valence of motivational drive
A fourth consideration is the valence of motivational drives that direct social interaction.216 Motivated behaviors regulating food intake are often distinguished as homeostatic (essential for maintaining energy balance and survival) or hedonic (driven by sensory perception or pleasure in the absence of a need state).217 Feeding behavior, therefore, is directed by motivational drives of opposing valence: the negative sensation of hunger and the positive hedonic value of palatable food.
In extrapolating to social behavior, equivalent opposing motivational drives may promote social interaction: the aversive state of isolation and the hedonic value of social reward. However, while mechanistic differences exist, the neural systems mediating homeostatic and hedonic feeding are proposed to be intertwined, and highly overlapping with reward circuitry.217, 218 Similarly, social reward circuitry is heavily recruited in isolated animals.
Engagement of reward circuitry in situations of social deficit may enhance the rewarding value of social contact—potentially similar to how food deprivation enhances the rewarding properties of food.219-222 In support of this concept, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in humans has revealed that more lonely individuals show greater activation of the ventral striatum in response to familiar social cues,223 but contrastingly reduced activation in response to unfamiliar social cues.224 Similarly, ventral striatal activity is initially high in response to palatable food but diminishes as individuals consume beyond satiety.225
The coordination of social behavior to meet homeostatic needs may, therefore, recruit both positive and negative motivational processes. The DRN dopamine system might be one source of negative motivational drive in response to social deficit.118 Recruitment of this system aligns with the “drive reduction” hypothesis, in which internal state elicits goal-directed behaviors in order to reduce the intensity of an aversive/negative motivational drive (e.g., hunger and thirst)226 (Fig. 1C). A potentially similar function has been described for arcuate nucleus AgRP neurons and nitric oxide synthase 1 (NOS1) neurons in the subfornical organ (SFO). These neurons show heightened activity during hunger (AgRP) and thirst (NOS1), their activity elicits an aversive state (place avoidance), and they are essential for driving feeding and drinking behaviors, respectively.21, 227
However, the role of valence processing in homeostatic feeding behavior is complex. AgRP neurons are activated in a state of energy deficit,228 and their optical stimulation can voraciously promote food consumption, but also elicits real-time place avoidance (indicative of an aversive state) in the absence of food.173, 227, 229 However, AgRP activity is rapidly suppressed on sensory detection of food,227, 228, 230 which, surprisingly, suggests that ongoing AgRP activity is dispensable for food consumption.
This paradox is potentially reconciled by the observation that brief optical stimulation, as little as 1 min, prior to food availability was sufficient to promote robust, sustained feeding in well-fed mice once food was made available.231 Furthermore, mice performed operant responses to stimulate AgRP neurons in the presence, but not the absence of food, suggesting that AgRP activity can be positively reinforcing.231
Therefore, an alternative hypothesis proposes that AgRP activity provides a sustained positive valence signal that potentiates the incentive value of food, and supports transition from foraging to feeding behavior via persistent changes in downstream circuitry.231 Intriguingly, this is not a feature of SFO NOS1 neurons, as prestimulation was insufficient to drive drinking behavior when water was subsequently made available.231 Therefore, the relationship between neuronal activity and behavioral regulation may depend on the specific homeostatic need.
While the precise role of DRN dopamine activity in social motivation and valence processing remains to be fully elucidated, drawing insight from other neural circuits that participate in maintaining homeostatic balance provides mechanistic clues into their mode of operation. However, an important consideration for socia
l behavior is that (unlike food detection) initial social contact does not necessarily guarantee a rewarding social experience. Therefore, immediate suppression of neural activity on social contact may be inappropriate for DRN dopamine neurons, and activity might persist until a stable relationship has been achieved. Moving forward, it will be important to determine the temporal dynamics of activity within and across neural circuits during the response to social deficit and to understand how valence is represented in these systems.
reference link : https://nyaspubs.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/nyas.14016
Source: The Conversation