Sleeping less than normal impacts how we feel the next morning.
“Not in the sense that we have more negative feelings, like being down or depressed. But participants in our study experienced a flattening of emotions when they slept less than normal.
They felt less joy, enthusiasm, attention and fulfilment,” says Associate Professor Ingvild Saksvik-Lehouillier at the Norwegian University of Science and Technology’s (NTNU) Department of Psychology.
Most sleep research is done in laboratories, but Saksvik-Lehouillier and the NTNU research team studied participants who slept at home.
The participants first spent seven nights in their own bed, and slept as long as they usually do.
Three of the mornings they conducted a set of tests. Next, the participants slept two hours less than normal for three nights. On two of the mornings they went through the same tests.
“We all have different sleep patterns. The point of having the participants sleep at home was to keep everything as similar to daily life as possible. In the imposed sleep deprivation phase, participants crawled under their covers two hours later than they normally did, and had to get up at their usual time,” says Saksvik-Lehouillier.
The practical testing took place about an hour and a half after the participants got up – and without having had any coffee.
They were shown 365 different pictures with random letters displayed on a computer screen over a period of 14 minutes. If the image did not contain the letter x, they were told to press the space bar, and if there was an x ??in the image, they were supposed to do nothing.
“We tested responsiveness and accuracy. The reaction time went down after the participants had been sleep deprived, but the error rate went up. It seems that we react more quickly to compensate for lower concentration. Then there’ll be more mistakes. It may be smart to avoid activities that require a high level of accuracy the morning after sleeping less than usual,” Saksvik-Lehouillier says.
Previous studies have shown that sleep deprivation may have about the same effect on driving as alcohol does.
Whereas the participants performed better and better each day they took the test after sleeping normally, they scored worse on accuracy each day after a night of insufficient sleep.
“We know that sleep is important for learning. Maybe that’s what we’re seeing here,” says Saksvik-Lehouillier.
In the second part of the test, participants answered a questionnaire to identify 20 positive and negative emotions.
“We didn’t find clear differences when it came to the negative emotions, but there were marked differences for the positive ones. Positive feelings scored worse after just one night of reduced sleep, and dropped even more after three nights.
I think this is a really interesting find. We already know that fewer positive emotions have a major impact on mental health. We also know that poor sleep is included in virtually all mental health diagnoses,” Saksvik-Lehouillier said.
In terms of how long the lack of good feelings lasts after sleep deprivation, she says the study did not address that, but the research team plans to investigate the duration of this state of mind.
It’s not just in Norway that people are sleeping less than they used to — it’s an international trend, especially for people who work full-time.
“It’s easy for us to go to bed later than we should, especially when we think, ‘I just have to finish watching this series.’ But we still have to get up to go to work, or study, or deliver our kids to kindergarten.
This contributes to getting too little sleep. How long we sleep is just part of the picture, but when we sleep is also important. An irregular circadian rhythm can be worse than sleeping too little. Going to bed and getting up at the same time is recommended,” says Saksvik-Lehouillier. She worries most about young people.
“Adolescents have a greater need for sleep and are a vulnerable group. They feel they need to be available online, there are a lot of entertainment temptations – and maybe the cell phone even goes to bed with them.
But they still have to get up the next morning to go to school. Sleep deprivation can quickly become an issue. Many teens experience sleep problems during exam times. For them, it may be a comfort to know that studies show that short-term lack of sleep doesn’t affect their ability to reflect and discuss what they’re learning.
Long-term studies conducted among shift workers who sleep too little over a long period of time show major negative consequences for their health, including a significantly increased risk for diseases such as cancer and diabetes.
“Sleep is individual. Not everyone needs to sleep seven and a half hours every night. And we’re A and B people. Some of us like to stay up till the wee hours, others love to rise and shine early in the morning. The most important thing is how you feel. If you’re in a good mood and alert when you get up, those are indications that your sleep habits are working for you,” says Saksvik-Lehouillier.
Shorter sleep times aren’t entirely negative either. The body compensates for shorter periods of time with more effective sleep. The time spent awake in bed is also less.
Study participants included 59 people between the ages of 18 and 35. The responsiveness and accuracy test is called Conners’ Continuous Performance Test-3.
WHAT IS SLEEP?
Sleep in mammals, as defined by its behavioural and physiological features, includes two distinct global activity states: rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and non-REM (NREM) sleep. These two states are typically characterized by electrophysiological measures including electroencephalography (EEG) and electromyography (EMG), as well as electrooculography in humans.
NREM sleep, which is further divided into four substages of increasing depth in humans, is characterized by high-amplitude, low-frequency EEG oscillations and behavioural quiescence with a relaxed muscle tone. Apart from the typical ocular movements, REM sleep (or paradoxical sleep) is characterized by low-amplitude, high-frequency EEG oscillation in association with muscle atonia. NREM and REM sleep alternate in cycles, with NREM sleep preceding REM sleep episodes.
In the mammalian brain, the sleep–wake cycle is orchestrated by a complex network of discrete neuronal populations that induce sleep or wakefulness via promoting or suppressing effects [1].
Nevertheless, some aspects of sleep homeostasis are different between humans and rodents. Human sleep is monophasic and typically consists of a single block of three to five cycles of sleep stages, usually happening during the night [1].
Despite variations in the daily duration of sleep and temporal pattern of vigilance states across laboratory rodents [2], sleep in these animals is generally polyphasic with repeated sleep episodes throughout the 24 h light–dark cycle [1].
In addition, commonly used laboratory rodents are nocturnal and preponderantly sleep during the light period [1].
WHY DO WE NEED TO SLEEP?
If the functions of sleep remain blurry, it has been proposed to play a key role in optimizing the conservation and utilization of energy by reallocating energy reserves to essential biological processes such as cellular maintenance, anabolism, immune function and neural plasticity rather than wake-related features such as vigilance, foraging and reproduction [3].
Furthermore, by overcoming the energy deficits accumulated during wakefulness and by preparing the organism for the next wake-related energy expenditure, sleep may further support fundamental mechanisms such as brain waste clearance via the glymphatic system [4] or daily stress resistance [5]. The disconnection from the environment during sleep has also been proposed to be critical for the homeostatic plasticity of the brain.
The synaptic homeostasis hypothesis suggests that the potentiation of synaptic connections throughout the brain that happens during wakefulness, supporting learning functions but also increasing the demand for energy and cellular supplies, is then normalized during sleep that can restore cellular homeostasis [6].
This re-normalization of synaptic strength could favour memory acquisition, consolidation and integration during sleep [6]. These interrelated phenomena have been described as vital for the organism, both ontogenetically and phylogenetically [7].
Indeed, the evidence that sleep serves essential functions is founded on several fundamental observations. First, sleep was spared by evolutionary pressures, indicating a crucial function, or functions, that cannot be easily circumvented [8].
Second, extended wakefulness beyond the normal sleep period necessarily leads to compensatory sleep rebound, underlying regulatory homeostatic mechanisms and the absolute need for a minimum amount of sleep [8]. Third, sleep loss has many harmful consequences for the organism, and, to date, investigations on the functions of sleep have mainly relied on studies of the consequences of sleep deprivation [8–10].
Finally, sleep is ubiquitous in the animal kingdom if one defines sleep to be simply a state of reduced activity, and/or reduced responsiveness to external stimuli [8]. However, in the absence of electrophysiological signs resembling those of mammals and birds, sleep in some animals such as reptiles, amphibians, fish and invertebrates may serve a function that is quite distinct from that served by human or mammalian sleep [8].
The noxious consequences of the lack of sleep have been known for a long time and have been used as a form of torture throughout history. However, the first reported experimental studies of sleep deprivation, especially the total absence of sleep, were published at the end the nineteenth century [11].
Conducted on dogs kept awake by constant activity or by using a bespoke cage keeping the animals awake without forced locomotion, they showed that total sleep loss led to ‘psychic exhaustion’, severe brain degenerations and was lethal after 4–17 days.
The second half of the nineteenth century also saw the emergence of clinical observations revealing the adverse effects of prolonged sleep deprivation and insomnia, provoking severe psychic disturbances such as delirium, hallucinations and emotional disruption [11,12].
Later studies on the effects of extended sleep loss in healthy people documented more detailed sleep-deprivation-induced psychopathological symptoms, including perceptual distortions, mood changes and psychosis [12].
These early studies shed light on the multiple deleterious effects of sleep loss, confirming the vital aspect of sleep and triggering the emergence of a growing interest in sleep in the scientific community. After the characterization of the homeostatic mechanisms underlying the stress response by Hans Selye in 1936 [13], some studies started to investigate the multiple facets of sleep, and in particular the consequences of sleep loss in the context of the so-called stress and coping processes [14,15].
IS SLEEP DEPRIVATION STRESSFUL?
Stress was initially defined as a ‘general adaptation syndrome’ whereby an organism reacts to nocuous stimuli by non-specific physiological or behavioural responses [16]. The main neuroendocrine systems involved in the stress response are the autonomic sympatho-adrenal system and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis [17].
In this review, we only focus on the latter because it is responsible for the core hormonal response to homeostatic challenge [18], as well as it being, by far, the most studied aspect of sleep deprivation in the context of stress.
During a stressful situation, the brain of mammals responds by activating the HPA axis, which releases corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) from the hypothalamus. When CRH reaches the pituitary, the latter releases adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH), which triggers the secretion in the bloodstream of the steroid hormone glucocorticoids by the adrenal cortex (cortisol in primates and corticosterone in rodents) [18].
Glucocorticoids are essential for the adaptive response to stress, ultimately leading to the normalization of glucocorticoid release by the inhibition of the HPA axis activity through a negative feedback signal [18]. The basal activity of the HPA axis, and therefore glucocorticoid release, is largely orchestrated by the internal circadian clock located in the suprachiasmatic nucleus of the hypothalamus and exhibits a daily rhythm in both humans and rodents [19,20].
The secretion of glucocorticoid peaks just before the end of the resting period and decreases throughout the active phase until reaching a trough at the beginning of the sleeping period [20]. Interestingly, the exogenous administration of CRH induces an increase in wakefulness and a reduction in NREM sleep in humans [21] and rodents [22,23], and inhibits sleep-active neurons in the preoptic area of the hypothalamus [22].
The reduction or the increase of corticosterone also alter sleep architecture in rats [24], and human studies showed that sleep itself slightly dampens cortisol release whereas arousal is associated with cortisol burst [25,26].
However, while an increase in CRH suppresses the homeostatic response following sleep deprivation, the change of circulating corticosterone concentration does not seem to affect sleep rebound [24].
In this context, it is no wonder that alterations in glucocorticoid secretion following sleep deprivation or restriction have been documented [27]. However, in humans, moderate sleep loss has modest effects on HPA axis activity, which is affected differently among studies. For instance, chronic sleep restriction of 5 h or more per night for four to eight consecutive nights does not alter the cortisol release pattern, most probably because of endogenous compensatory mechanisms during sleep [28–31].
However, greater sleep loss characterized by a maximum of 4 h of sleep per night for one to six consecutive nights has been associated with dampened morning cortisol awakening response and increased afternoon/evening cortisol level [32–37], as well as reduced reactivity and slower recovery of the cortisol response [38].
Furthermore, a longitudinal study showed that recurrent short sleep is associated with an adverse cortisol secretion pattern [39]. The effect of sleep restriction on cortisol release seems to be dependent on when sleep loss occurs during the night and if subjects are usually awake or asleep at the time of sleep deprivation [40,41], highlighting the influence of complex interactions between sleep pressure and the subject’s endogenous circadian and ultradian rhythms on HPA axis activity [42].
The flattening of the circadian release of cortisol has also been observed after one night of total sleep deprivation [43], despite most of the reports describing either an increase [33,44–47] or a decrease [48–52] in cortisol level following sleep loss. The same disparity is observed with sleep fragmentation and REM sleep deprivation [53,54].
These discrepancies could be explained by possible methodological limitations which might prevent an accurate assessment of the effect of sleep deprivation on cortisol release rhythmicity. However, and interestingly, some studies also failed to show a change in the HPA axis activity following total sleep deprivation when the experimental conditions were designed to avoid stress [55–57].
This shed light on the potential confounding effects of stress per se in sleep-deprivation experiments. Indeed, in addition to the immediate effects of sleep deprivation on the HPA axis activity, sleep loss has also been shown to affect subsequent stress reactivity.
If one night of sleep deprivation did not increase responsiveness to an acute psychosocial challenge despite increased stress levels [58,59], poor sleep habits were associated with higher blood pressure and cortisol level during psychosocial stress [60]. Sleep quality seems to play a more important role in stress reactivity than sleep quantity [61].
The increase in HPA axis activity that is exhibited by some sleep-deprived individuals might be the direct consequence, at least in part, of the mental and physical load the subjects experience in order to stay awake rather than the effect of sleep loss alone. One could even consider sleep deprivation as a stressor sensu stricto (strictly speaking) in the event of a stress response beyond what is expected during relaxed wakefulness.
This is no trivial matter considering the complex and reciprocal relationship between sleep and HPA axis activity [62,63], in addition to the well-established effects of stress on sleep homeostasis [64,65]. Whether the activation of the neuroendocrine stress system is the result of forced wakefulness or the stressful nature of the sleep-deprivation procedure is a fundamental question, and the absence of a clear answer has serious consequences for sleep research involving sleep deprivation.
This is particularly true in animals, for whom sleep deprivation is necessarily achieved by subjecting them to potentially stressful movement-restricting novel environments or external stimulation [9]. Several sleep-deprivation protocols for rodents have been designed and used during the last 50 years, and understanding their specificities is crucial for the development of novel procedures to suppress sleep while minimizing, and even eliminating, any stress-associated confounding effects on the experimental outputs.
DO SLEEP-DEPRIVATION PROCEDURES IN RODENTS AFFECT THE LEVEL OF STRESS?
With the growth of sleep science from the 1970s and given the acknowledged, but poorly understood, bidirectional link between sleep and stress, some studies thus started to control for the stress-associated effects of their sleep-deprivation procedures in rodents as they became the model of choice for sleep studies [66,67].
Thereafter, the increasingly documented effect of sleep loss on the activity of the HPA axis, measured by the level of stress hormones, provided insight into the complex interrelated processes underlying sleep homeostasis and stress.
From the 1960s onward, several automated methods based on forced locomotion emerged in an attempt to standardize sleep-deprivation protocols. Treadmills or rotating wheels that can either move continuously or as soon as the animal displays behavioural and/or electrophysiological signs of sleep were used in rats [68,69].
However, after spending some time in the apparatus, the animals often find strategies to get some rest, such as running in the opposite direction to that of the treadmill or wheel and having short sleep episodes before waking up to avoid falling. If the amount of sleep suppression obviously depends on the movement speed imposed on the animals, it has been shown that an animal could sleep on average almost 40% of the time [70].
More importantly, major concerns have been raised about the confounding effects of exercise and fatigue on output measures [27], and particularly their associated stressful components. Using a rotating cylinder, an early study showed that 21.5 h of sleep deprivation in rats caused no significant increase in corticosterone compared with control animals with ad libitum (as much and as often as desired) sleep [71].
However, the release of corticosterone was greatly increased after 20 min of forced locomotion. Despite this study’s conclusion stating that the overall low level of stress was unlikely to substantially alter sleep patterns during recovery, the immediate but transient effects of forced locomotion on corticosterone release, and thus subsequent effects on sleep parameters and underlying sleep-regulating mechanisms, cannot be ignored.
Furthermore, other studies have shown that sleep deprivation, fragmentation or restriction by forced locomotion for 11–48 h in rats markedly increased the release of corticosterone [72–77]. In mice, 24 h of sleep deprivation using an activity wheel did not cause a significant increase in corticosterone [78], while another study showed that chronic sleep interruption during 14 days using a rotating drum induced an increase in corticosterone [79].
More recently, other paradigms have been developed in order to minimize the stress and/or physical fatigue of previous forced locomotion sleep-deprivation protocols. An apparatus consisting of two platforms alternatively moving below and above a water surface was designed to keep mice in constant motion without locomotion [80].
However, 10 h of total sleep deprivation using this novel paradigm did not prevent a significant rise of corticosterone [81]. Other methods, based on real-time EEG or EMG biofeedback systems triggering the rotation of cylindrical cages or running wheels when NREM sleep, REM sleep or inactivity are detected, were able to prevent sleep for 6–11 h in male rats and mice with a mild but non-significant increase in corticosterone [77,82].
The disc-over-water (DOW) apparatus was introduced in order to better control the influence of physical stimuli on experimental and control rats or mice [83–85]. In this paradigm, a sleep-deprived animal and its yoked control counterpart are housed on each side of a two-chambered cage separated by a central wall.
The floor of the cage consists of a single flat disc, suspended over a tank of water, so that each animal lay on one half of the disc. When behavioural or electrophysiological signs of sleep are observed in the experimental individual, the disc rotates around the central axis of the apparatus and forces both animals to walk in the opposite direction in order to avoid falling into the water.
This method ensures similar physical stimulation and locomotor activity for both experimental and control animals, while allowing the latter to sleep whenever the experimental one is awake and the disc immobile. However, the yoked control is inevitably partially sleep deprived when attempting to sleep at the same time as its experimental counterpart, with a respective reduction of total sleep time of around 30% and 90% [86].
The DOW paradigm is usually performed over an extended period of time, ranging from 2 days to one month. When not leading to the death of the experimental animals, the DOW applied for several days induced a severe stress syndrome including a decrease in body weight, enlarged adrenals and stomach ulcers, despite a reduction in corticosterone compared with baseline in male rats [85].
The decrease or the absence of alteration of the corticosterone level following sleep restriction by the DOW paradigm has also been observed in other studies in male rats [87–89]. However, male rats subjected to the DOW paradigm for days in order to achieve total sleep deprivation exhibited an increase in ACTH and corticosterone, while displaying only an increase in ACTH when deprived of REM sleep specifically [83].
In addition, one study showed that an increase in corticosterone level can be observed after 2 days of sleep deprivation in female rats compared with control animals that were not subjected to the DOW device [90].
Other protocols based on the same principles as the DOW, but without water, were also developed. In these apparatuses, experimental and yoked male mice subjected to a 9-day experiment displayed a significant increase in corticosterone only during the first day of sleep deprivation [91], while male rats sleep-deprived during 12 h by means of a gradual increase in the speed and the variability of the disc’s rotation exhibited a slight increase in brain corticosterone concentration, but this did not exceed the normal circadian peak [92].
Other automated approaches involving tactile stimulations have been employed to fragment sleep in mice. A cage with a sweeping horizontal bar just above the floor was used to achieve 6 h of sleep fragmentation without altering corticosterone level [93], but later studies showed that the same procedure applied for 1–3 days induced an increase in corticosterone [94,95]. However, 3 days of sleep fragmentation using a rotating lever providing tactile stimulations did not seem to increase circulating corticosterone concentration in adolescent mice [96].
Nowadays, one of the most popular methods to achieve a total suppression of sleep in rodents is the ‘gentle handling’ (GH) procedure, in which behavioural and/or electrophysiological signs of sleep are actively monitored by a trained experimenter who keeps the animal awake using gentle sensory stimulations [97–99].
When properly performed, the GH procedure can suppress almost all sleep activity [98]. The GH method has never been formally standardized across laboratories and encompasses a variety of different stimulations. Animals can be kept awake using external stimuli such as gentle tapping and/or shaking of the cage, mild noises, bedding disturbance or by touching the animal with a brush or directly with the hand [98,99].
Despite having been initially designed to minimize the spurious consequences of stress on sleep, the effects of GH on the HPA axis activity is more controversial than the consensually recognized stressful effects of sleep-deprivation protocols based on forced locomotion. The exposure to external stimuli between 2 and 24 h has been shown to induce an increase in corticosterone levels in male mice [100–105] and rats [106–110], as well as for very short periods of time in neonatal rats [110].
However, this increase was usually mild and within the range of normal amplitude observed during the undisturbed period. Standardizing GH protocols is not an easy task, as it is often necessary to adapt the amount of stimulation depending on the sleepiness and the drowsiness of each subject, which inevitably introduces variability.
The number of stimulations applied to the animal typically increases throughout the sleep-deprivation procedure [103]. It has been shown that mice kept awake with as little disturbance as possible display lower corticosterone levels than animals kept awake using social stimuli or regular direct handling [111,112].
If rats seem to be less affected by stress during GH procedures [109,113], conflicting results have been reported regarding the habituation of mice to GH. Compared with undisturbed animals, 3 min of daily GH during 6 days in male mice induced no alteration in corticosterone release during the first day then an increase in corticosterone by the end of the sixth day [114], while the exact opposite was also described [115].
In addition, the familiarity an animal has with the experimenter can influence the consistency of the experimental results [116], while exposure to male experimenters is associated with a higher physiological stress response than exposure to female experimenters [117].
An alternative milder procedure to GH, aiming at inducing sleep deprivation by spontaneous exploratory and locomotor behaviour in rodents using novel objects or nesting material, has also been proposed [118–121].
Despite minimizing the number of external stimuli as well as allowing a more natural and spontaneous arousal state, the ‘novel objects’ sleep-deprivation paradigm performed during 4 h in male rats has been shown to increase free corticosterone in hippocampal dialysates [122].
Furthermore, the presence of novel objects may be associated with learning effects [123]. Of note, the introduction of novel objects in the cage of sleep-deprived animals is most of the time used in combination with more classical GH techniques [101,113].
The inverted ‘flowerpot’ method was originally established in the 1960s to suppress REM sleep in cats [124] and was later adapted to rodents [125,126]. The experimental animal is placed on a small platform emerging from a water tank, while the control animal is usually placed on a larger platform.
The size of the small platform allows the animal to squat and enter into NREM sleep. However, at the onset of REM sleep and associated muscle relaxation, the animal loses its balance and falls into the water, causing it to awaken. By contrast, the control individual can sleep ad libitum on the larger platform. The stressful aspects of this procedure have been raised in a number of studies, and an early report showed that male rats deprived of REM sleep (and food) for 24 h exhibited an increase in corticosterone and gastric ulceration [127].
Later studies also highlighted that selective REM-sleep deprivation for 1–5 days using the single platform method induces an increase in corticosterone and ACTH in male rats [128–135]. REM-sleep restriction for three consecutive days, 6 h daily, also induced an increase in corticosterone.
Recently, a study showed that the administration of the single platform method on male mice for 1 day induced an increase in corticosterone, while 3 days of REM-sleep deprivation led to the death of some animals [94], further emphasizing the noxious aspects of the method.
A modified version of the classic ‘flowerpot’ method with multiple platforms in a larger tank, allowing the sleep deprivation of several animals at the same time, was later designed in order to remove stress-associated movement restriction and isolation [136]. An early study assessing adrenal hypertrophy, thymus atrophy, body weight loss and stomach ulceration induced by 3 days of sleep deprivation using the single and multiple platforms methods in male rats showed that, if both paradigms only caused mild stress compared with food deprivation, the multiple platforms protocol was less stressful than the classical platform technique [137].
However, another study also comparing the effects of the single and multiple platforms methods, applied during 4 days, on the HPA axis activity of male rats showed that both protocols caused an increased secretion of corticosterone and ACTH, and confirmed that this effect was stronger in the single platform paradigm [138].
The stressful aspect of the multiple platforms apparatus was further highlighted in several other studies showing that 18 h to 21 days of REM-sleep suppression induced an increase in corticosterone and/or ACTH in male rats [139–148] and mice [149,150]. Other related methods using two small platforms or a ‘grid over water’ have been shown to also induce an increase in corticosterone in male mice after 2 and 3 days, respectively, [151,152].
In juvenile and male rats subjected to REM-sleep restriction for 14 or 18 h daily during a maximum of 21 consecutive days using the multiple platforms technique, a higher level of corticosterone has been observed than in non-sleep-restricted animals [153,154].
However, female rats that were REM sleep-restricted for 20 h daily for 6 days during pregnancy did not exhibit any increase in corticosterone release compared with control animals, while having higher relative adrenal weight [155].
The absence of any change in corticosterone release following REM-sleep deprivation for 2–3 days in the multiple platforms apparatus has also been observed in male and female rats [156,157].
One major limitation in most studies assessing the effect of forced wakefulness on stress through the activity of the HPA axis is that the level of stress-related hormones is usually measured at the end of the sleep-deprivation procedure.
As several studies demonstrated that sleep deprivation using different paradigms can induce an early and transient increase in corticosterone before normalization [71,158–160], this is likely to mask any early increase in corticosterone that could potentially have an influence on later output measures.
Source:
NTNU
REFERENCES
1. Weber F, Dan Y. 2016. Circuit-based interrogation of sleep control. Nature 538, 51–59. (10.1038/nature19773) [PubMed]
2. Franken P, Malafosse A, Tafti M. 1999. Genetic determinants of sleep regulation in inbred mice. Sleep 22, 155–169. [PubMed]
3. Schmidt MH. 2014. The energy allocation function of sleep: a unifying theory of sleep, torpor, and continuous wakefulness. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 47, 122–153. (10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.08.001) [PubMed]
4. Albrecht U, Ripperger JA. 2018. Circadian clocks and sleep: impact of rhythmic metabolism and waste clearance on the brain. Trends Neurosci. 41, 677–688. (10.1016/j.tins.2018.07.007) [PubMed]
5. Blaxton JM, Bergeman CS, Whitehead BR, Braun ME, Payne JD. 2017. Relationships among nightly sleep quality, daily stress, and daily affect. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 72, 363–372.
6. Tononi G, Cirelli C. 2014. Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron 81, 12–34. (10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.025)
7. Bringmann H. 2019. Genetic sleep deprivation: using sleep mutants to study sleep functions. EMBO Rep. 20, e46807 (10.15252/embr.201846807)
8. Cirelli C, Tononi G. 2008. Is sleep essential? PLoS Biol. 6, e216 (10.1371/journal.pbio.0060216)
9. Colavito V, Fabene PF, Grassi-Zucconi G, Pifferi F, Lamberty Y, Bentivoglio M, Bertini G. 2013. Experimental sleep deprivation as a tool to test memory deficits in rodents. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7, 106 (10.3389/fnsys.2013.00106)
10. Longordo F, Kopp C, Luthi A. 2009. Consequences of sleep deprivation on neurotransmitter receptor expression and function. Eur. J. Neurosci. 29, 1810–1819. (10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06719.x) [PubMed]
11. Bentivoglio M, Grassi-Zucconi G. 1997. The pioneering experimental studies on sleep deprivation. Sleep 20, 570–576. (10.1093/sleep/20.7.570) [PubMed]
12. Waters F, Chiu V, Atkinson A, Blom JD. 2018. Severe sleep deprivation causes hallucinations and a gradual progression toward psychosis with increasing time awake. Front. Psychiatry 9, 303 (10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00303)
13. Selye H. 1998. A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. [Reprinted from Nature 1936; 138, 32.]. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 10, 230–231. (10.1176/jnp.10.2.230a) [PubMed]
14. Mark J, Heiner L, Mandel P, Godin Y. 1969. Norepinephrine turnover in brain and stress reactions in rats during paradoxical sleep deprivation. Life Sci. 8, 1085–1093. (10.1016/0024-3205(69)90161-1) [PubMed]
15. Kollar EJ, Slater GR, Palmer JO, Doctor RF, Mandell AJ. 1966. Stress in subjects undergoing sleep deprivation. Psychosom. Med. 28, 101–113. (10.1097/00006842-196603000-00002) [PubMed]
16. Selye H. 1950. Stress and the general adaptation syndrome. Br. Med. J. 1, 1383–1392. (10.1136/bmj.1.4667.1383)
17. Kvetnansky R, Pacak K, Fukuhara K, Viskupic E, Hiremagalur B, Nankova B, Goldstein DS, Sabban EL, Kopin IJ. 1995. Sympathoadrenal system in stress. Interaction with the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical system. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 771, 131–158. (10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb44676.x) [PubMed]
18. Herman JP, McKlveen JM, Ghosal S, Kopp B, Wulsin A, Makinson R, Scheimann J, Myers B. 2016. Regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical stress response. Compr. Physiol. 6, 603–621. (10.1002/cphy.c150015)
19. Son GH, Cha HK, Chung S, Kim K. 2018. Multimodal regulation of circadian glucocorticoid rhythm by central and adrenal clocks. J. Endocr. Soc. 2, 444–459. (10.1210/js.2018-00021)
20. Kalsbeek A, van der Spek R, Lei J, Endert E, Buijs RM, Fliers E. 2012. Circadian rhythms in the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis. Mol. Cell. Endocrinol. 349, 20–29. (10.1016/j.mce.2011.06.042) [PubMed]
21. Born J, Spath-Schwalbe E, Schwakenhofer H, Kern W, Fehm HL. 1989. Influences of corticotropin-releasing hormone, adrenocorticotropin, and cortisol on sleep in normal man. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 68, 904–911. (10.1210/jcem-68-5-904) [PubMed]
22. Gvilia I, Suntsova N, Kumar S, McGinty D, Szymusiak R. 2015. Suppression of preoptic sleep-regulatory neuronal activity during corticotropin-releasing factor-induced sleep disturbance. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 309, R1092–R1100. (10.1152/ajpregu.00176.2015) [PubMed]
23. Romanowski CP, Fenzl T, Flachskamm C, Wurst W, Holsboer F, Deussing JM, Kimura M. 2010. Central deficiency of corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor type 1 (CRH-R1) abolishes effects of CRH on NREM but not on REM sleep in mice. Sleep 33, 427–436. (10.1093/sleep/33.4.427)
24. Bradbury MJ, Dement WC, Edgar DM. 1998. Effects of adrenalectomy and subsequent corticosterone replacement on rat sleep state and EEG power spectra. Am. J. Physiol. 275, R555–R565. (10.1152/ajpcell.1998.275.2.C555) [PubMed]
25. Spath-Schwalbe E, Gofferje M, Kern W, Born J, Fehm HL. 1991. Sleep disruption alters nocturnal ACTH and cortisol secretory patterns. Biol. Psychiatry. 29, 575–584. (10.1016/0006-3223(91)90093-2) [PubMed]
26. Follenius M, Brandenberger G, Bandesapt JJ, Libert JP, Ehrhart J. 1992. Nocturnal cortisol release in relation to sleep structure. Sleep 15, 21–27. (10.1093/sleep/15.1.21) [PubMed]
27. Meerlo P, Sgoifo A, Suchecki D. 2008. Restricted and disrupted sleep: effects on autonomic function, neuroendocrine stress systems and stress responsivity. Sleep Med. Rev. 12, 197–210. (10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.007) [PubMed]
28. Pejovic S, et al. 2013. Effects of recovery sleep after one work week of mild sleep restriction on interleukin-6 and cortisol secretion and daytime sleepiness and performance. Am. J. Physiol. Endocrinol. Metab. 305, E890–E896. (10.1152/ajpendo.00301.2013)
29. Leproult R, Van Cauter E. 2011. Effect of 1 week of sleep restriction on testosterone levels in young healthy men. JAMA 305, 2173–2174. (10.1001/jama.2011.710)
30. Voderholzer U, Piosczyk H, Holz J, Feige B, Loessl B, Kopasz M, Riemann D, Nissen C. 2012. The impact of increasing sleep restriction on cortisol and daytime sleepiness in adolescents. Neurosci. Lett. 507, 161–166. (10.1016/j.neulet.2011.12.014) [PubMed]
31. Nedeltcheva AV, Kessler L, Imperial J, Penev PD. 2009. Exposure to recurrent sleep restriction in the setting of high caloric intake and physical inactivity results in increased insulin resistance and reduced glucose tolerance. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 94, 3242–3250. (10.1210/jc.2009-0483)
32. Guyon A, Balbo M, Morselli LL, Tasali E, Leproult R, L’Hermite-Baleriaux M, Van Cauter E, Spiegel K. 2014. Adverse effects of two nights of sleep restriction on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in healthy men. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 99, 2861–2868. (10.1210/jc.2013-4254)
33. Leproult R, Copinschi G, Buxton O, Van Cauter E. 1997. Sleep loss results in an elevation of cortisol levels the next evening. Sleep 20, 865–870. [PubMed]
34. Reynolds AC, Dorrian J, Liu PY, Van Dongen HP, Wittert GA, Harmer LJ, Banks S. 2012. Impact of five nights of sleep restriction on glucose metabolism, leptin and testosterone in young adult men. PLoS ONE 7, e41218 (10.1371/journal.pone.0041218)
35. Spiegel K, Leproult R, L’Hermite-Baleriaux M, Copinschi G, Penev PD, Van Cauter E. 2004. Leptin levels are dependent on sleep duration: relationships with sympathovagal balance, carbohydrate regulation, cortisol, and thyrotropin. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 89, 5762–5771. (10.1210/jc.2004-1003) [PubMed]
36. Spiegel K, Leproult R, Van Cauter E. 1999. Impact of sleep debt on metabolic and endocrine function. Lancet 354, 1435–1439. (10.1016/S0140-6736(99)01376-8) [PubMed]
37. Omisade A, Buxton OM, Rusak B. 2010. Impact of acute sleep restriction on cortisol and leptin levels in young women. Physiol. Behav. 99, 651–656. (10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.01.028) [PubMed]
38. Guyon A, Morselli LL, Balbo ML, Tasali E, Leproult R, L’Hermite-Baleriaux M, Van Cauter E, Spiegel K. 2017. Effects of insufficient sleep on pituitary-adrenocortical response to CRH stimulation in healthy men. Sleep 40, zsx064 (10.1093/sleep/zsx064)
39. Abell JG, Shipley MJ, Ferrie JE, Kivimaki M, Kumari M. 2016. Recurrent short sleep, chronic insomnia symptoms and salivary cortisol: a 10-year follow-up in the Whitehall II study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 68, 91–99. (10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.02.021)
40. Weitzman ED, Zimmerman JC, Czeisler CA, Ronda J. 1983. Cortisol secretion is inhibited during sleep in normal man. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 56, 352–358. (10.1210/jcem-56-2-352) [PubMed]
41. Wu H, Zhao Z, Stone WS, Huang L, Zhuang J, He B, Zhang P, Li Y. 2008. Effects of sleep restriction periods on serum cortisol levels in healthy men. Brain Res. Bull. 77, 241–245. (10.1016/j.brainresbull.2008.07.013) [PubMed]
42. Leproult R, Colecchia EF, L’Hermite-Baleriaux M, Van Cauter E. 2001. Transition from dim to bright light in the morning induces an immediate elevation of cortisol levels. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 86, 151–157. (10.1210/jc.86.1.151) [PubMed]
43. Gil-Lozano M, Hunter PM, Behan LA, Gladanac B, Casper RF, Brubaker PL. 2016. Short-term sleep deprivation with nocturnal light exposure alters time-dependent glucagon-like peptide-1 and insulin secretion in male volunteers. Am. J. Physiol. Endocrinol. Metab. 310, E41–E50. (10.1152/ajpendo.00298.2015) [PubMed]
44. Schussler P, Uhr M, Ising M, Weikel JC, Schmid DA, Held K, Mathias S, Steiger A. 2006. Nocturnal ghrelin, ACTH, GH and cortisol secretion after sleep deprivation in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, 915–923. (10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.05.002) [PubMed]
45. Chapotot F, Buguet A, Gronfier C, Brandenberger G. 2001. Hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis activity is related to the level of central arousal: effect of sleep deprivation on the association of high-frequency waking electroencephalogram with cortisol release. Neuroendocrinology 73, 312–321. (10.1159/000054648) [PubMed]
46. von Treuer K, Norman TR, Armstrong SM. 1996. Overnight human plasma melatonin, cortisol, prolactin, TSH, under conditions of normal sleep, sleep deprivation, and sleep recovery. J. Pineal Res. 20, 7–14. (10.1111/j.1600-079X.1996.tb00232.x) [PubMed]
47. Benedict C, Hallschmid M, Lassen A, Mahnke C, Schultes B, Schioth HB, Born J, Lange T. 2011. Acute sleep deprivation reduces energy expenditure in healthy men. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 93, 1229–1236. (10.3945/ajcn.110.006460) [PubMed]
48. Vgontzas AN, Mastorakos G, Bixler EO, Kales A, Gold PW, Chrousos GP. 1999. Sleep deprivation effects on the activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal and growth axes: potential clinical implications. Clin. Endocrinol. (Oxf.) 51, 205–215. (10.1046/j.1365-2265.1999.00763.x) [PubMed]
49. Cedernaes J, Osler ME, Voisin S, Broman JE, Vogel H, Dickson SL, Zierath JR, Schioth HB,, Benedict C. 2015. Acute sleep loss induces tissue-specific epigenetic and transcriptional alterations to circadian clock genes in men. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 100, E1255–E1261. (10.1210/JC.2015-2284) [PubMed]
50. Klumpers UM, Veltman DJ, van Tol MJ, Kloet RW, Boellaard R, Lammertsma AA, Hoogendijk WJG. 2015. Neurophysiological effects of sleep deprivation in healthy adults, a pilot study. PLoS ONE 10, e0116906 (10.1371/journal.pone.0116906)
51. Vargas I, Lopez-Duran N. 2017. The cortisol awakening response after sleep deprivation: is the cortisol awakening response a ‘response’ to awakening or a circadian process? J. Health Psychol. 1359105317738323 (10.1177/1359105317738323) [PubMed]
52. Trivedi MS, Holger D, Bui AT, Craddock TJA, Tartar JL. 2017. Short-term sleep deprivation leads to decreased systemic redox metabolites and altered epigenetic status. PLoS ONE 12, e0181978 (10.1371/journal.pone.0181978)
53. Born J, Schenk U, Spath-Schwalbe E, Fehm HL. 1988. Influences of partial REM sleep deprivation and awakenings on nocturnal cortisol release. Biol. Psychiatry 24, 801–811. (10.1016/0006-3223(88)90256-9) [PubMed]
54. Stamatakis KA, Punjabi NM. 2010. Effects of sleep fragmentation on glucose metabolism in normal subjects. Chest 137, 95–101. (10.1378/chest.09-0791)
55. Chennaoui M, et al. 2011. Effect of one night of sleep loss on changes in tumor necrosis factor alpha (TNF-alpha) levels in healthy men. Cytokine 56, 318–324. (10.1016/j.cyto.2011.06.002) [PubMed]
56. Pejovic S, Vgontzas AN, Basta M, Tsaoussoglou M, Zoumakis E, Vgontzas A, Bixler E, Chrousos G. 2010. Leptin and hunger levels in young healthy adults after one night of sleep loss. J. Sleep Res. 19, 552–558. (10.1111/j.1365-2869.2010.00844.x)
57. Honma A, Revell VL, Gunn PJ, Davies SK, Middleton B, Raynaud FI, Skene DJ. 2020. Effect of acute total sleep deprivation on plasma melatonin, cortisol and metabolite rhythms in females. Eur. J. Neurosci. 51, 366–378. (10.1111/ejn.14411)
58. Schwarz J, Gerhardsson A, van Leeuwen W, Lekander M, Ericson M, Fischer H, Kecklund G, Åkerstedt T. 2018. Does sleep deprivation increase the vulnerability to acute psychosocial stress in young and older adults? Psychoneuroendocrinology 96, 155–165. (10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.06.003) [PubMed]
59. Vargas I, Lopez-Duran N. 2017. Investigating the effect of acute sleep deprivation on hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal-axis response to a psychosocial stressor. Psychoneuroendocrinology 79, 1–8. (10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.01.030) [PubMed]
60. Massar SAA, Liu JCJ, Mohammad NB, Chee MWL. 2017. Poor habitual sleep efficiency is associated with increased cardiovascular and cortisol stress reactivity in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 81, 151–156. (10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.04.013) [PubMed]
61. Bassett SM, Lupis SB, Gianferante D, Rohleder N, Wolf JM. 2015. Sleep quality but not sleep quantity effects on cortisol responses to acute psychosocial stress. Stress 18, 638–644. (10.3109/10253890.2015.1087503)
62. Steiger A. 2002. Sleep and the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical system. Sleep Med. Rev. 6, 125–138. (10.1053/smrv.2001.0159) [PubMed]
63. Buckley TM, Schatzberg AF. 2005. On the interactions of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and sleep: normal HPA axis activity and circadian rhythm, exemplary sleep disorders. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 90, 3106–3114. (10.1210/jc.2004-1056) [PubMed]
64. Pawlyk AC, Morrison AR, Ross RJ, Brennan FX. 2008. Stress-induced changes in sleep in rodents: models and mechanisms. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 99–117. (10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.06.001)
65. Kim EJ, Dimsdale JE. 2007. The effect of psychosocial stress on sleep: a review of polysomnographic evidence. Behav. Sleep Med. 5, 256–278. (10.1080/15402000701557383)
66. Mendelson W, Guthrie RD, Guynn R, Harris RL, Wyatt RJ. 1974. Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep deprivation, stress and intermediary metabolism. J. Neurochem. 22, 1157–1159. (10.1111/j.1471-4159.1974.tb04353.x) [PubMed]
67. Stern WC, Miller FP, Cox RH, Maickel RP. 1971. Brain norepinephrine and serotonin levels following REM sleep deprivation in the rat. Psychopharmacologia 22, 50–55. (10.1007/BF00401466) [PubMed]
68. Stefurak SJ, Stefurak ML, Mendelson WB, Gillin JC, Wyatt RJ. 1977. A method for sleep depriving rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 6, 137–139. (10.1016/0091-3057(77)90169-1) [PubMed]
69. Levitt RA. 1966. Sleep deprivation in the rat. Science 153, 85–87. (10.1126/science.153.3731.85) [PubMed]
70. Ferguson J, Dement W. 1967. The effect of variations in total sleep time on the occurrence of rapid eye movement sleep in cats. Electroencephalogr Clin. Neurophysiol. 22, 2–10. (10.1016/0013-4694(67)90003-X) [PubMed]
71. Tobler I, Murison R, Ursin R, Ursin H, Borbely AA. 1983. The effect of sleep deprivation and recovery sleep on plasma corticosterone in the rat. Neurosci. Lett. 35, 297–300. (10.1016/0304-3940(83)90333-6) [PubMed]
72. Roman V, Hagewoud R, Luiten PG, Meerlo P. 2006. Differential effects of chronic partial sleep deprivation and stress on serotonin-1A and muscarinic acetylcholine receptor sensitivity. J. Sleep Res. 15, 386–394. (10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00555.x) [PubMed]
73. Campbell IG, Guinan MJ, Horowitz JM. 2002. Sleep deprivation impairs long-term potentiation in rat hippocampal slices. J. Neurophysiol. 88, 1073–1076. (10.1152/jn.2002.88.2.1073) [PubMed]
74. Tartar JL, Ward CP, Cordeira JW, Legare SL, Blanchette AJ, McCarley RW, Strecker RE. 2009. Experimental sleep fragmentation and sleep deprivation in rats increases exploration in an open field test of anxiety while increasing plasma corticosterone levels. Behav. Brain Res. 197, 450–453. (10.1016/j.bbr.2008.08.035)
75. Sgoifo A, Buwalda B, Roos M, Costoli T, Merati G, Meerlo P. 2006. Effects of sleep deprivation on cardiac autonomic and pituitary-adrenocortical stress reactivity in rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, 197–208. (10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.06.009) [PubMed]
76. Meerlo P, Koehl M, van der Borght K, Turek FW. 2002. Sleep restriction alters the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal response to stress. J. Neuroendocrinol. 14, 397–402. (10.1046/j.0007-1331.2002.00790.x) [PubMed]
77. McCarthy A, Loomis S, Eastwood B, Wafford KA, Winsky-Sommerer R, Gilmour G. 2017. Modelling maintenance of wakefulness in rats: comparing potential non-invasive sleep-restriction methods and their effects on sleep and attentional performance. J. Sleep Res. 26, 179–187. (10.1111/jsr.12464) [PubMed]
78. Dispersyn G, Sauvet F, Gomez-Merino D, Ciret S, Drogou C, Leger D, Gallopin T, Chennaoui M. 2017. The homeostatic and circadian sleep recovery responses after total sleep deprivation in mice. J. Sleep Res. 26, 531–538. (10.1111/jsr.12541) [PubMed]
79. Feng L, Wu HW, Song GQ, Lu C, Li YH, Qu LN, Chen S, Liu X, Chang Q. 2016. Chronical sleep interruption-induced cognitive decline assessed by a metabolomics method. Behav. Brain Res. 302, 60–68. (10.1016/j.bbr.2015.12.039) [PubMed]
80. Pierard C, et al. 2007. Modafinil restores memory performance and neural activity impaired by sleep deprivation in mice. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 88, 55–63. (10.1016/j.pbb.2007.07.006) [PubMed]
81. Pierard C, Liscia P, Chauveau F, Coutan M, Corio M, Krazem A, Beracochea D. 2011. Differential effects of total sleep deprivation on contextual and spatial memory: modulatory effects of modafinil. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 97, 399–405. (10.1016/j.pbb.2010.09.016) [PubMed]
82. Fenzl T, Romanowski CP, Flachskamm C, Honsberg K, Boll E, Hoehne A, Kimura M. 2007. Fully automated sleep deprivation in mice as a tool in sleep research. J. Neurosci. Methods. 166, 229–235. (10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.07.007) [PubMed]
83. Bergmann BM, Everson CA, Kushida CA, Fang VS, Leitch CA, Schoeller DA, Refetoff S, Rechtschaffen A. 1989. Sleep deprivation in the rat: V. Energy use and mediation. Sleep 12, 31–41. (10.1093/sleep/12.1.31) [PubMed]
84. Rechtschaffen A, Bergmann BM. 1995. Sleep deprivation in the rat by the disk-over-water method. Behav. Brain Res. 69, 55–63. (10.1016/0166-4328(95)00020-T) [PubMed]
85. Rechtschaffen A, Gilliland MA, Bergmann BM, Winter JB. 1983. Physiological correlates of prolonged sleep deprivation in rats. Science 221, 182–184. (10.1126/science.6857280) [PubMed]
86. Everson CA, Bergmann BM, Rechtschaffen A. 1989. Sleep deprivation in the rat: III. Total sleep deprivation. Sleep 12, 13–21. (10.1093/sleep/12.1.13) [PubMed]
87. Everson CA, Crowley WR. 2004. Reductions in circulating anabolic hormones induced by sustained sleep deprivation in rats. Am. J. Physiol. Endocrinol. Metab. 286, E1060–E1070. (10.1152/ajpendo.00553.2003) [PubMed]
88. Everson CA, Szabo A. 2011. Repeated exposure to severely limited sleep results in distinctive and persistent physiological imbalances in rats. PLoS ONE 6, e22987 (10.1371/journal.pone.0022987)
89. Lopez-Rodriguez F, Kim J, Poland RE. 2004. Total sleep deprivation decreases immobility in the forced-swim test. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 1105–1111. (10.1038/sj.npp.1300406) [PubMed]
90. Yang CK, Tsai HD, Wu CH, Cho CL. 2019. The role of glucocorticoids in ovarian development of sleep deprived rats. Taiwan. J. Obstet. Gynecol. 58, 122–127. (10.1016/j.tjog.2018.11.023) [PubMed]
91. Ringgold KM, Barf RP, George A, Sutton BC, Opp MR. 2013. Prolonged sleep fragmentation of mice exacerbates febrile responses to lipopolysaccharide. J. Neurosci. Methods. 219, 104–112. (10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.07.008)
92. Leenaars CH, Dematteis M, Joosten RN, Eggels L, Sandberg H, Schirris M, Feenstra MGP, Van Someren EJW. 2011. A new automated method for rat sleep deprivation with minimal confounding effects on corticosterone and locomotor activity. J. Neurosci. Methods. 196, 107–117. (10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.01.014) [PubMed]
93. Ramesh V, Kaushal N, Gozal D. 2009. Sleep fragmentation differentially modifies EEG delta power during slow wave sleep in socially isolated and paired mice. Sleep Sci. 2, 64–75,
94. Yonglin G, Brandon A, Michael BR, Rif SE-M. 2017. Corticosterone response in sleep deprivation and sleep fragmentation. J. Sleep Disord. Manage. 3, 018 (10.23937/2572-4053.1510018)
95. Dumaine JE, Ashley NT. 2015. Acute sleep fragmentation induces tissue-specific changes in cytokine gene expression and increases serum corticosterone concentration. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 308, R1062–R1069. (10.1152/ajpregu.00049.2015)
96. Wallace E, et al. 2015. Differential effects of duration of sleep fragmentation on spatial learning and synaptic plasticity in pubertal mice. Brain Res. 1615, 116–128. (10.1016/j.brainres.2015.04.037) [PubMed]
97. Endo T, Schwierin B, Borbely AA, Tobler I. 1997. Selective and total sleep deprivation: effect on the sleep EEG in the rat. Psychiatry Res. 66, 97–110. (10.1016/S0165-1781(96)03029-6) [PubMed]
98. Franken P, Dijk DJ, Tobler I, Borbely AA. 1991. Sleep deprivation in rats: effects on EEG power spectra, vigilance states, and cortical temperature. Am. J. Physiol. 261, R198–R208. [PubMed]
99. Tobler I, Deboer T, Fischer M. 1997. Sleep and sleep regulation in normal and prion protein-deficient mice. J. Neurosci. 17, 1869–1879. (10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-05-01869.1997)
100. Mongrain V, Hernandez SA, Pradervand S, Dorsaz S, Curie T, Hagiwara G, Gip P, Heller HC, Franken P. 2010. Separating the contribution of glucocorticoids and wakefulness to the molecular and electrophysiological correlates of sleep homeostasis. Sleep 33, 1147–1157. (10.1093/sleep/33.9.1147)
101. Sanchez-Alavez M, Conti B, Moroncini G, Criado JR. 2007. Contributions of neuronal prion protein on sleep recovery and stress response following sleep deprivation. Brain Res. 1158, 71–80. (10.1016/j.brainres.2007.05.010)
102. Palchykova S, Winsky-Sommerer R, Meerlo P, Durr R, Tobler I. 2006. Sleep deprivation impairs object recognition in mice. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 85, 263–271. (10.1016/j.nlm.2005.11.005) [PubMed]
103. Hagewoud R, Havekes R, Novati A, Keijser JN, Van der Zee EA, Meerlo P. 2010. Sleep deprivation impairs spatial working memory and reduces hippocampal AMPA receptor phosphorylation. J. Sleep Res. 19, 280–288. (10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00799.x) [PubMed]
104. Naidoo N, Davis JG, Zhu J, Yabumoto M, Singletary K, Brown M, Galante R, Agarwal B, Baur JA. 2014. Aging and sleep deprivation induce the unfolded protein response in the pancreas: implications for metabolism. Aging Cell 13, 131–141. (10.1111/acel.12158)
105. Onaolapo JO, Onaolapo YA, Akanmu AM, Olayiwola G. 2016. Caffeine and sleep-deprivation mediated changes in open-field behaviours, stress response and antioxidant status in mice. Sleep Sci. 9, 236–243. (10.1016/j.slsci.2016.10.008)
106. Kawakami M, Kimura F, Tsai CW. 1983. Correlation of growth hormone secretion to sleep in the immature rat. J. Physiol. 339, 325–337. (10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014719)
107. Zant JC, Leenaars CH, Kostin A, Van Someren EJ, Porkka-Heiskanen T. 2011. Increases in extracellular serotonin and dopamine metabolite levels in the basal forebrain during sleep deprivation. Brain Res. 1399, 40–48. (10.1016/j.brainres.2011.05.008) [PubMed]
108. Gip P, Hagiwara G, Sapolsky RM, Cao VH, Heller HC, Ruby NF. 2004. Glucocorticoids influence brain glycogen levels during sleep deprivation. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 286, R1057–R1062. (10.1152/ajpregu.00528.2003) [PubMed]
109. Melgarejo-Gutierrez M, Acosta-Pena E, Venebra-Munoz A, Escobar C, Santiago-Garcia J, Garcia-Garcia F. 2013. Sleep deprivation reduces neuroglobin immunoreactivity in the rat brain. Neuroreport 24, 120–125. (10.1097/WNR.0b013e32835d4b74) [PubMed]
110. Hairston IS, Ruby NF, Brooke S, Peyron C, Denning DP, Heller HC, Sapolsky RM. 2001. Sleep deprivation elevates plasma corticosterone levels in neonatal rats. Neurosci. Lett. 315, 29–32. (10.1016/S0304-3940(01)02309-6) [PubMed]
111. Meerlo P, Turek FW. 2001. Effects of social stimuli on sleep in mice: non-rapid-eye-movement (NREM) sleep is promoted by aggressive interaction but not by sexual interaction. Brain Res. 907, 84–92. (10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02603-8) [PubMed]
112. Kopp C, Longordo F, Nicholson JR, Luthi A. 2006. Insufficient sleep reversibly alters bidirectional synaptic plasticity and NMDA receptor function. J. Neurosci. 26, 12 456–12 465. (10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2702-06.2006)
113. Kalinchuk AV, McCarley RW, Porkka-Heiskanen T, Basheer R. 2010. Sleep deprivation triggers inducible nitric oxide-dependent nitric oxide production in wake-active basal forebrain neurons. J. Neurosci. 30, 13 254–13 264. (10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0014-10.2010)
114. Longordo F, Fan J, Steimer T, Kopp C, Luthi A. 2011. Do mice habituate to ‘gentle handling?’ A comparison of resting behavior, corticosterone levels and synaptic function in handled and undisturbed C57BL/6. J. Mice. Sleep 34, 679–681. (10.1093/sleep/34.5.679)
115. Vecsey CG, Wimmer ME, Havekes R, Park AJ, Perron IJ, Meerlo P, Abel T, 2013. Daily acclimation handling does not affect hippocampal long-term potentiation or cause chronic sleep deprivation in mice. Sleep 36, 601–607. (10.5665/sleep.2556)
116. van Driel KS, Talling JC. 2005. Familiarity increases consistency in animal tests. Behav. Brain Res. 159, 243–245. (10.1016/j.bbr.2004.11.005) [PubMed]
117. Sorge RE, et al. 2014. Olfactory exposure to males, including men, causes stress and related analgesia in rodents. Nat. Methods 11, 629–632. (10.1038/nmeth.2935) [PubMed]
118. Cirelli C, Faraguna U, Tononi G. 2006. Changes in brain gene expression after long-term sleep deprivation. J. Neurochem. 98, 1632–1645. (10.1111/j.1471-4159.2006.04058.x) [PubMed]
119. Cirelli C, Tononi G. 2000. Differential expression of plasticity-related genes in waking and sleep and their regulation by the noradrenergic system. J. Neurosci. 20, 9187–9194. (10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-24-09187.2000)
120. Gompf HS, Mathai C, Fuller PM, Wood DA, Pedersen NP, Saper CB, Lu J. 2010. Locus ceruleus and anterior cingulate cortex sustain wakefulness in a novel environment. J. Neurosci. 30, 14 543–14 551. (10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3037-10.2010)
121. Bellesi M, de Vivo L, Chini M, Gilli F, Tononi G, Cirelli C. 2017. Sleep loss promotes astrocytic phagocytosis and microglial activation in mouse cerebral cortex. J. Neurosci. 37, 5263–5273. (10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3981-16.2017)
122. Penalva RG, Lancel M, Flachskamm C, Reul JM, Holsboer F, Linthorst AC. 2003. Effect of sleep and sleep deprivation on serotonergic neurotransmission in the hippocampus: a combined in vivo microdialysis/EEG study in rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 17, 1896–1906. (10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02612.x) [PubMed]
123. Kopp C, Longordo F, Luthi A. 2007. Experience-dependent changes in NMDA receptor composition at mature central synapses. Neuropharmacology 53, 1–9. (10.1016/j.neuropharm.2007.03.014) [PubMed]
124. Jouvet D, Vimont P, Delorme F. 1964. Study of selective deprivation of the paradoxal phase of sleep in the cat. J. Physiol. (Paris) 56, 381. [PubMed]
125. Mendelson WB, Guthrie RD, Frederick G, Wyatt RJ. 1974. The flower pot technique of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep deprivation. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2, 553–556. (10.1016/0091-3057(74)90018-5) [PubMed]
126. Morden B, Mitchell G, Dement W. 1967. Selective REM sleep deprivation and compensation phenomena in the rat. Brain Res. 5, 339–349. (10.1016/0006-8993(67)90042-X) [PubMed]
127. Murison R, Ursin R, Coover GD, Lien W, Ursin H. 1982. Sleep deprivation procedure produces stomach lesions in rats. Physiol. Behav. 29, 693–694. (10.1016/0031-9384(82)90240-2) [PubMed]
128. Galvão MD, Sinigaglia-Coimbra R, Kawakami SE, Tufik S, Suchecki D. 2009. Paradoxical sleep deprivation activates hypothalamic nuclei that regulate food intake and stress response. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1176–1183. (10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.03.003) [PubMed]
129. Kim EY, Mahmoud GS, Grover LM. 2005. REM sleep deprivation inhibits LTP in vivo in area CA1 of rat hippocampus. Neurosci. Lett. 388, 163–167. (10.1016/j.neulet.2005.06.057) [PubMed]
130. Machado RB, Tufik S, Suchecki D. 2008. Chronic stress during paradoxical sleep deprivation increases paradoxical sleep rebound: association with prolactin plasma levels and brain serotonin content. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 1211–1224. (10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.06.007) [PubMed]
131. Mathangi DC, Shyamala R, Subhashini AS. 2012. Effect of REM sleep deprivation on the antioxidant status in the brain of Wistar rats. Ann. Neurosci. 19, 161–164. (10.5214/ans.0972.7531.190405)
132. Sallanon-Moulin M, Touret M, Didier-Bazes M, Roudier V, Fages C, Tardy M, Jouvet M. 1994. Glutamine synthetase modulation in the brain of rats subjected to deprivation of paradoxical sleep. Brain Res. Mol. Brain Res. 22, 113–120. (10.1016/0169-328X(94)90038-8) [PubMed]
133. Youngblood BD, Zhou J, Smagin GN, Ryan DH, Harris RB. 1997. Sleep deprivation by the ‘flower pot’ technique and spatial reference memory. Physiol. Behav. 61, 249–256. (10.1016/S0031-9384(96)00363-0) [PubMed]
134. Mirescu C, Peters JD, Noiman L, Gould E. 2006. Sleep deprivation inhibits adult neurogenesis in the hippocampus by elevating glucocorticoids. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103, 19 170–19 175. (10.1073/pnas.0608644103)
135. Machado RB, Tufik S, Suchecki D. 2013. Role of corticosterone on sleep homeostasis induced by REM sleep deprivation in rats. PLoS ONE 8, e63520 (10.1371/journal.pone.0063520)
136. van Hulzen ZJ, Coenen AM. 1981. Paradoxical sleep deprivation and locomotor activity in rats. Physiol. Behav. 27, 741–744. (10.1016/0031-9384(81)90250-X) [PubMed]
137. Coenen AM, van Luijtelaar EL. 1985. Stress induced by three procedures of deprivation of paradoxical sleep. Physiol. Behav. 35, 501–504. (10.1016/0031-9384(85)90130-1) [PubMed]
138. Suchecki D, Tiba PA, Tufik S. 2002. Paradoxical sleep deprivation facilitates subsequent corticosterone response to a mild stressor in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 320, 45–48. (10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00024-1) [PubMed]
139. Andersen ML, Bignotto M, Machado RB, Tufik S. 2004. Different stress modalities result in distinct steroid hormone responses by male rats. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 37, 791–797. (10.1590/S0100-879X2004000600003) [PubMed]
140. Brianza-Padilla M, Sanchez-Munoz F, Vazquez-Palacios G, Huang F, Almanza-Perez JC, Bojalil R, Bonilla-Jaime H. 2018. Cytokine and microRNA levels during different periods of paradoxical sleep deprivation and sleep recovery in rats. PeerJ 6, e5567 (10.7717/peerj.5567)
141. Ma C, Wu G, Wang Z, Wang P, Wu L, Zhu G, Zhao H. 2014. Effects of chronic sleep deprivation on the extracellular signal-regulated kinase pathway in the temporomandibular joint of rats. PLoS ONE 9, e107544 (10.1371/journal.pone.0107544)
142. Palma BD, Suchecki D, Tufik S. 2000. Differential effects of acute cold and footshock on the sleep of rats. Brain Res. 861, 97–104. (10.1016/S0006-8993(00)02024-2) [PubMed]
143. Suchecki D, Tufik S. 2000. Social stability attenuates the stress in the modified multiple platform method for paradoxical sleep deprivation in the rat. Physiol. Behav. 68, 309–316. (10.1016/S0031-9384(99)00181-X) [PubMed]
144. Mueller AD, Pollock MS, Lieblich SE, Epp JR, Galea LA, Mistlberger RE. 2008. Sleep deprivation can inhibit adult hippocampal neurogenesis independent of adrenal stress hormones. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 294, R1693–R1703. (10.1152/ajpregu.00858.2007) [PubMed]
145. Solanki N, Atrooz F, Asghar S, Salim S. 2016. Tempol protects sleep-deprivation induced behavioral deficits in aggressive male Long-Evans rats. Neurosci. Lett. 612, 245–250. (10.1016/j.neulet.2015.12.032)
146. Zhang L, et al. 2017. Melatonin prevents sleep deprivation-associated anxiety-like behavior in rats: role of oxidative stress and balance between GABAergic and glutamatergic transmission. Am. J. Transl. Res. 9, 2231–2242.
147. Tiba PA, Oliveira MG, Rossi VC, Tufik S, Suchecki D. 2008. Glucocorticoids are not responsible for paradoxical sleep deprivation-induced memory impairments. Sleep 31, 505–515. (10.1093/sleep/31.4.505)
148. Suchecki D, Lobo LL, Hipolide DC, Tufik S. 1998. Increased ACTH and corticosterone secretion induced by different methods of paradoxical sleep deprivation. J. Sleep Res. 7, 276–281. (10.1046/j.1365-2869.1998.00122.x) [PubMed]
149. De Lorenzo BH, de Oliveira Marchioro L, Greco CR, Suchecki D. 2015. Sleep-deprivation reduces NK cell number and function mediated by beta-adrenergic signalling. Psychoneuroendocrinology 57, 134–143. (10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.04.006) [PubMed]
150. Mueller AD, Parfyonov M, Pavlovski I, Marchant EG, Mistlberger RE. 2014. The inhibitory effect of sleep deprivation on cell proliferation in the hippocampus of adult mice is eliminated by corticosterone clamp combined with interleukin-1 receptor 1 knockout. Brain Behav. Immun. 35, 182–188. (10.1016/j.bbi.2013.10.001) [PubMed]
151. Arthaud S, Varin C, Gay N, Libourel PA, Chauveau F, Fort P, Luppi P-H, Peyron C. 2015. Paradoxical (REM) sleep deprivation in mice using the small-platforms-over-water method: polysomnographic analyses and melanin-concentrating hormone and hypocretin/orexin neuronal activation before, during and after deprivation. J. Sleep Res. 24, 309–319. (10.1111/jsr.12269) [PubMed]
152. Chanana P, Kumar A. 2016. GABA-BZD receptor modulating mechanism of Panax quinquefolius against 72-h sleep deprivation induced anxiety like behavior: possible roles of oxidative stress, mitochondrial dysfunction and neuroinflammation. Front. Neurosci. 10, 84 (10.3389/fnins.2016.00084)
153. Han C, Li F, Ma J, Liu Y, Li W, Mao Y, Song Y, Guo S, Liu J. 2017. Distinct behavioral and brain changes after different durations of the modified multiple platform method on rats: an animal model of central fatigue. PLoS ONE 12, e0176850 (10.1371/journal.pone.0176850)
154. da Silva Rocha-Lopes J, Machado RB, Suchecki D. 2018. Chronic REM sleep restriction in juvenile male rats induces anxiety-like behavior and alters monoamine systems in the amygdala and hippocampus. Mol. Neurobiol. 55, 2884–2896. (10.1007/s12035-017-0541-3) [PubMed]
155. Pardo GV, Goularte JF, Hoefel AL, de Castro AL, Kucharski LC, da Rosa Araujo AS, Lucion AB. 2016. Effects of sleep restriction during pregnancy on the mother and fetuses in rats. Physiol. Behav. 155, 66–76. (10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.11.037) [PubMed]
156. Hajali V, Sheibani V, Esmaeili-Mahani S, Shabani M. 2012. Female rats are more susceptible to the deleterious effects of paradoxical sleep deprivation on cognitive performance. Behav. Brain Res. 228, 311–318. (10.1016/j.bbr.2011.12.008) [PubMed]
157. Hajali V, Sheibani V, Ghazvini H, Ghadiri T, Valizadeh T, Saadati H, Shabani M. 2015. Effect of castration on the susceptibility of male rats to the sleep deprivation-induced impairment of behavioral and synaptic plasticity. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 123, 140–148. (10.1016/j.nlm.2015.05.008) [PubMed]
158. Cohen S, Kozlovsky N, Matar MA, Kaplan Z, Zohar J, Cohen H. 2012. Post-exposure sleep deprivation facilitates correctly timed interactions between glucocorticoid and adrenergic systems, which attenuate traumatic stress responses. Neuropsychopharmacology 37, 2388–2404. (10.1038/npp.2012.94)
159. Porkka-Heiskanen T, Smith SE, Taira T, Urban JH, Levine JE, Turek FW, Stenberg D. 1995. Noradrenergic activity in rat brain during rapid eye movement sleep deprivation and rebound sleep. Am. J. Physiol. 268, R1456–R1463. (10.1152/ajpregu.1995.268.6.R1456) [PubMed]
160. Porter NM, et al. 2012. Hippocampal CA1 transcriptional profile of sleep deprivation: relation to aging and stress. PLoS ONE 7, e40128 (10.1371/journal.pone.0040128)